Cannabis Indica

Page 1
The second edition of The Great Psychotherapy Debate has been updated and revised
to expand the presentation of the Contextual Model, which is derived from a
scientific understanding of how humans heal in a social context and explains
findings from a vast array of psychotherapy studies. This model provides a
compelling alternative to traditional research on psychotherapy, which tends to
focus on identifying the most effective treatment for particular disorders through
emphasizing the specific ingredients of treatment. The new edition also includes
a history of healing practices, medicine, and psychotherapy; an examination of
therapist effects; and a thorough review of the research on common factors such
as the alliance, expectations, and empathy.
Bruce E. Wampold, PhD, ABPP, is the Patricia L. Wolleat Professor of Coun-
seling Psychology at the University of Wisconsin–Madison and director of the
Research Institute at Modum Bad Psychiatric Center in Vikersund, Norway.
Zac E. Imel, PhD, is an assistant professor with the counseling psychology
program in the department of educational psychology and an adjunct assistant
professor in the Department of Psychiatry at the University of Utah.
The Great Psychotherapy Debate
This page intentionally left blank
The Evidence for What Makes
Psychotherapy Work
Second Edition
Bruce E. Wampold and
Zac E. Imel
The Great Psychotherapy
Debate
Second edition published 2015
by Routledge
711 Third Avenue, New York, NY 10017
and by Routledge
27 Church Road, Hove, East Sussex BN3 2FA
Routledge is an imprint of the Taylor & Francis Group, an informa business
© 2015 Bruce E. Wampold and Zac E. Imel
The right of Bruce E. Wampold and Zac E. Imel to be identified as authors
of this work has been asserted by them in accordance with sections 77
and 78 of the Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988.
All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reprinted or reproduced
or utilised in any form or by any electronic, mechanical, or other means,
now known or hereafter invented, including photocopying and recording,
or in any information storage or retrieval system, without permission in
writing from the publishers.
Trademark notice: Product or corporate names may be trademarks
or registered trademarks, and are used only for identification and
explanation without intent to infringe.
First edition published by Routledge 2001
Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data
Wampold, Bruce E., 1948–
The great psychotherapy debate : the evidence for what makes
psychotherapy work / by Bruce E. Wampold and Zac E. Imel. —
Second edition.
pages cm
Includes bibliographical references and index.
1. Psychotherapy—Philosophy. 2. Psychotherapy—Evaluation.
I. Imel, Zac E. II. Title.
RC437.5.W35 2015
616.89′14—dc23
2014032867
ISBN: 978-0-8058-5708-5 (hbk)
ISBN: 978-0-8058-5709-2 (pbk)
ISBN: 978-0-203-58201-5 (ebk)
Typeset in Baskerville
by Apex CoVantage, LLC
To all of my collaborators, personal and professional, many
of whom have made sacrifices so that this revision could be
completed. BEW
To KT, Jiajia, and Lulu. ZEI
This page intentionally left blank
Contents
Preface
viii
1 History of Medicine, Methods, and Psychotherapy:
Progress and Omissions
1
2 The Contextual Model: Psychotherapy as a Socially
Situated Healing Practice
37
3 Contextual Model Versus Medical Model: Choosing
a Progressive Research Programme
62
4 Absolute Efficacy: The Benefits of Psychotherapy
Established by Meta-Analysis
82
5 Relative Efficacy: The Dodo Bird Still Gets It
114
6 Therapist Effects: An Ignored but Critical Factor
158
7 General Effects: Surviving Challenges and Anticipating
Additional Evidence
178
8 Specific Effects: Where Are They?
213
9 Beyond the Debate: Implications of the Research
Synthesis for Theory, Policy, and Practice
255
References
279
Index
317
When one publishes a book with the word debate in the title, one must be pre-
pared for rebuttals. In science, rebuttals are best presented as evidence. In the
13 years since the first edition, there have been many arguments about what
makes psychotherapy work, best characterized by a debate between proponents
of evidence-based treatments and proponents of common factors. This debate
often mischaracterizes one side or the other, with more than a few instances
when rhetorical accusations have predominated, rather than evidence.
Rhetoric does not keep me up at night but evidence surely does. And since
the first edition, there have been many reasons for sleepless nights. As Zac
and I discuss in this volume, psychotherapy evidence has proliferated since the
first edition. The number of psychotherapy clinical trials and meta-analyses
of these trials has increased exponentially. There is more evidence now for the
effectiveness of psychotherapy than ever before. Would that evidence trend
toward showing the scientific folly of the Contextual Model that I proposed
in 2001? If so, then the Contextual Model would fall on the scrap heap of
perfectly rational, but empirically unsupported, theories, including chemical
theories of fermentation (spontaneous generation), light propagation through
aether, and a static universe (Einstein’s Universe). Yet, the research conducted
in the last decade and a half has not produced evidence that seriously threatens
the Contextual Model—indeed, the evidence for the Contextual Model is an
order of magnitude stronger than it was in 2001.
In the preface to the first edition I spoke about the meaning of psychother-
apy for me personally and dedicated the book in part to my therapist. Sadly,
some have used this intimate story to say that my work is biased and should
not be trusted. So, let me be clear about this—as is true of all humans, I do
indeed have biases. However, one hallmark of science is that we intentionally
put our biases aside and attend rationally to evidence. Moreover, the scientific
endeavor is a correcting system in that evidence in the end will prevail and
theories will be abandoned, despite their ability to attract adherents, should the
evidence be sufficiently compelling. As with all theories, the current iteration
of the Contextual Model will be modified as anomalies are detected—in the
coming decades, evidence will likely emerge that both clarifies and complicates
Preface
Preface ix
components of the model. In this process, my allegiance is to the evidence and
there is no disgrace in having one’s theory ultimately to be found in the same
dustbin as Einstein’s Static Universe.
This edition of The Great Psychotherapy Debate differs from the first edition in
several ways. Of course, the research corpus is updated and the various chap-
ters reflect the latest evidence. The first chapter now presents a brief history
of medicine and psychotherapy to put the current debate into proper perspec-
tive. In 2001, the Contextual Model I proposed was just emerging from the
work of Jerome Frank. During the last decade, the model has expanded based
on social science research—the expanded model is presented in Chapter 2 .
As in the previous edition, there is a chapter ( Chapter 3 ) that presents what
evidence is to be considered and then discusses the conjectures of the Medical
Model and the Contextual Model. As in the first edition, there are chapters
that examine the evidence for absolute efficacy ( Chapter 4 ), relative efficacy
( Chapter 5 ), and therapists’ effects ( Chapter 6 ). In the first edition, evidence
related to general effects was limited to a discussion of the therapeutic alliance.
We have expanded this section to also include how placebos induce powerful
expectations, as well as several other therapeutic factors hypothesized to be
powerful in the Contextual Model ( Chapter 7 ). Chapter 8 reviews the literature
on the importance of specific ingredients. Chapter 9 makes conclusions related
to theory, practice, and policy.
Books have authors. But authorship reflects an amalgamation of influences.
To a large extent, my work was spawned from discussions with students and
collaborations with colleagues around the world. Zac Imel, from his first days
as my doctoral advisee over a decade ago, has challenged me to think deeply
about the issues discussed in this edition and expand my methodological exper-
tise. He would bring articles and books to me: “You have to read this!” and “We
have to learn new methods to understand this issue,” his restless mind collect-
ing and synthesizing information from a variety of spheres. This edition has
continued our intellectual collaboration, mutually stimulating and rewarding.
BEW, Madison, Wisconsin, April 1, 2014
Quite unintentionally, my psychology training began in small groups that
were a part of church youth camps in the Red Rock Canyons of Oklahoma. I
observed the work of talented group leaders who worked to replace emptiness
and shame with acceptance and support. While many of my peers were taken
with spiritual explanations for these experiences, in me they awoke an apprecia-
tion for open and emotionally charged relationships and provided an enduring
template that continues to guide my relationships and inform my clinical work.
The intervention we discuss in this book is still mostly a human conversation—
perhaps the ultimate in low technology. Something in the core of human connec-
tion and interaction has the power to heal. Ironically, the unavoidable complexity
of unstructured, emotional dialogue poses an immense challenge to scientists
who wish to know why it is that conversations with certain characteristics lead to
x Preface
improvements in psychological well-being, decreases in distress, and recovery from
profoundly disabling mental health problems—while other conversations do not.
As we complete this second attempt to summarize the existing evidence for a
general model of psychotherapy as outlined by the Contextual Model, we are
confronted with interesting times for psychotherapy as a science and profession.
Patients prefer psychotherapy as a first-line treatment for many problems, but
psychotherapy continues to decrease as an overall percentage of mental health
care. There is more evidence for the effectiveness of what therapists do and
how they do it than ever before, but much remains unknown. Technology has
revolutionized almost every aspect of human life, transforming science, medi-
cine, entertainment, journalism, and social interaction. However, our current
gold standard for evaluating the process of change in psychotherapy—human
behavioral coding of patient–provider interactions—is based on 70-year old
technology first used by Carl Rogers and his students. Simultaneously, computer
scientists and electrical engineers have developed techniques that can model the
words in all published books and automatically recognize speech from acoustic
signal. The American Psychological Association released a general statement
on the effectiveness of psychotherapy, but many contend that advocating for
the effectiveness of psychotherapy generally is like talking about the effective-
ness of “drugs.” Instead, they argue we have scores of specific, evidenced-based
treatments with demonstrated effectiveness. The Veterans Health Administra-
tion launched one of the largest psychotherapy quality improvement initiatives
in history by disseminating specific psychotherapies into mental health specialty
clinics, but regular monitoring of patient outcomes or provider behavior is
mostly absent in community settings.
I am the son and grandson of accountants, engineers, and teachers, and
thus it is not surprising that my rebellion into the practice of psychotherapy
led back to an immersion in numbers and the academy. I first read The Great
Psychotherapy Debate after graduating from a small liberal arts college where I
thrived in the intellectual space between scientifically oriented psychology and
a pluralistic religious studies department with professors who often shared
lunch (and maybe a polite argument or two). In psychotherapy, I was frustrated
by what I saw as a glut of “true believers” and the persistence of theoretical
camps that seemed independent of the evidence. Thus, I was quickly taken
by the parsimony of the common factors approach outlined in the book and
Bruce’s devotion to data and the scientific method. Upon arrival in Madison
in 2003, I quickly began what I have come to recognize as an unusually close
and productive collaboration, working and thinking about how to make sense
of the beautiful mess that is psychotherapy data. Bruce encouraged my natural
skepticism and curiosity, and Monday morning espressos were a time to poke
holes in our own theories (as well as those of others, of course). I like to think
my contributions to this volume began during those meetings.
ZEI, Salt Lake City, Utah, April 2014
Examining only the current state of affairs in any field reveals recent trends but
can obscure other critical issues. And often it is what is left behind in our efforts
to attain progress that reveals much about the field. Psychotherapy, shaped by
context, actors, and allied fields (particularly medicine), is no exception. The
pursuit of progress—or maybe better said, the inevitable process of progress—
comes with a cost. It may well be that what is cast off as archaic is actually the
essence, and what is retained is a façade. On the other hand, innovation and
progress can be achieved, and nostalgically clinging to the past can be damag-
ing. In this book, a critical examination of the progress in psychotherapy is
undertaken with attention to the hidden, forgotten, and ignored factors, as well
as to the current practices, policies, and research.
A romantic notion is that progress is a result of knowledge—evidence driv-
ing innovation and practice. However, the modern view is that events are the
result of human action, and human action is influenced by a myriad of factors,
of which evidence is only one. But then, even the notion of evidence is prob-
lematic, as evidence is the interpretation of a pattern of data—interpretation is
a human cognitive task subject to biases, power, methods, and constraints. The
social sciences are particularly vulnerable to these vectors as precision is not
great, replication is infrequent, and the subject matter is imbedded in cultural,
political, and fiscal contexts. In most instances, psychotherapy exists within a
health delivery system that exerts further pressure to progress along a narrow
corridor. So constrained, psychotherapy exists as it is, but its future is to be deter-
mined, primarily by the actors whose influence is most boldly exerted. This
book’s thesis, which is outside the canonical view yet built upon the very evi-
dence collected within that canon, provides an alternative course for the future.
The course, which was abandoned some time ago and is updated here under
the name “The Contextual Model,” may, if we can be so bold, be the one that
has more potential to benefit patients than the course presently being pursued.
Before the evidence can be presented for a contextual view of psychotherapy,
there are certain consequences from history that need to be fully understood.
How did we get here? And what was omitted to make certain progressions? Sev-
eral intertwined stories need to be examined: the stories of medicine, research
Chapter 1
History of Medicine, Methods,
and Psychotherapy
Progress and Omissions
2 History of Medicine, Methods, and Psychotherapy
methods (particularly clinical trials), and psychotherapy. Of course, each of these
histories could fill a volume on their own (indeed, there are several volumes on
each), but abbreviated versions are sufficient to take notice of important elements.
Medicine
Medicine is the dominant healing practice in Western cultures. It is the appli-
cation of scientific knowledge to cure disease, alleviate physical suffering, and
prolong life. However, modern medicine is a recent invention and one that
evolved from a tradition of healing practices, most of which medicine would
rather not claim as antecedents.
The Origins of Medicine as a Healing Practice
Healing practices appear in the earliest humans and characterize, in an impor-
tant way, the essential nature of humanness:
According to Sir William Osler (1932), the desire to take medicine is one
feature that distinguishes hominids from their fellow creatures. . . . Although
nothing is known about the earliest medications or about the first physi-
cian, historians date the earliest portrait of a physician to Cro-Magnon
times, 20,000 B.C. (Haggard, 1934; Bromberg, 1954). This horned tailed,
hirsute, and animal-like apparition had great psychological effect, and it is
likely that the treatment used was simply a vehicle for the psychological or
placebo effect and was without any intrinsic merit (Model, 1955).
(Shapiro & Shapiro, 1997b, p. 3)
Indeed, it is impossible to identify historically a civilization in which med-
icines, rituals, and healers were not central features of the culture (Shapiro
& Shapiro, 1997b; Wilson, 1978). As societies evolved, the human mind was
predisposed to generate explanations of physical, mental, and somatic phe-
nomena (Gardner, 1998)—the particular explanations differed by culture and
have evolved over time—but the art of using the explanations to create and
apply treatments—that is, the practice of healing—has spanned both culture
and time. Indeed, the nature of the healing practice is a large component of
the description of any culture, as healing and other cultural practices are so
intertwined. The Pythagoreans suggested that the body was composed of
four humors (viz., blood, phlegm, yellow bile, and black bile), and personali-
ties were manifestations of various mixtures of humors; illness resulted when
the humors, which were thought to be affected by diet, weather, and climate,
were unbalanced (Morris, 1997; Shapiro & Shapiro, 1997b; Wampold, 2001a).
The Apache shaman, elaborately dressed in animal skins and masks and whose
power derived from special status among the spirits or from possession of a
sacred object, administered rituals involving dance, drums, rattles, prayers, and
History of Medicine, Methods, and Psychotherapy 3
chants and to replace evils spirits with protective ones (Morris, 1997). Tradi-
tional Chinese medical practices, described in The I Ching (Book of Changes)
and the Huang Ti Nei Ching Su Wen (The Yellow Emperor’s Classic of Internal
Medicine), postulated five elements: water, fire, wood, metal, and earth and
combinations of the yin and yang; diseases were treated with five tastes, five
types of grain, and five flavors (e.g., pungent food was used to prevent disinte-
gration of the liver and sour food to drain the liver), supplemented by acupunc-
ture, which has persisted as a Chinese treatment for more than 2,500 years
(Shapiro & Shapiro, 1997b). Each with a medical explanation for its effect, the
pharmacopoeias of European medicine of the seventeenth century contained
such substances as Vigo’s plaster (viper’s flesh with live worms and frogs), fox
lungs, moss from the skulls of victims of violent death, Gascoigne’s powder
(bezoar, amber, pearls, crab’s eyes and claws, and coral), human urine, various
sexual organs, excreta (from many different sources), human placenta, saliva
from fasting individuals, and wood lice (Shapiro & Shapiro, 1997b).
There is no attempt to romanticize ancient or indigenous medical practices,
as it is clear that many such healing practices were ineffective and some danger-
ous (Shapiro & Shapiro, 1997a,b). Hippocrates prescribed a diet that excluded
vegetables and fruits, resulting in vitamin deficiencies. Acupuncture, due to
unsterilized needles, caused homologous serum jaundice, a deadly disease that
was prevalent in China for centuries and killed many. Dehydrating procedures,
such as bloodletting, vomiting, enemas, and leeches “killed more patients than
any other treatment in the history of medicine” (Shapiro & Shapiro, 1997a,
p. 18). Indeed, George Washington was no doubt killed by his physician, who
treated his abscessed tonsil with a variety of procedures that exacerbated the
natural dehydration of fever. Nevertheless, effective or ineffective, cultures
developed explanations for illness and developed treatments—each explanation
and the associated treatment were consistent with the beliefs and practices of
the culture and in many ways were defining features of the society.
Although the origins of Western scientific medicine can be traced to the
ancient Greeks, the preponderance of the treatments in Europe and the United
States remained ineffective, by modern standards of medicine, until at least the
nineteenth century. The introduction of the twined concepts of materialism
and specificity, arising in the Renaissance era, along with the concept of the
placebo, allowed modern medicine to ride the crest of the wave created by sci-
ence and the scientific method.
Materialism, Specificity, and the Placebo as Critical
Concepts of Modern Medicine: The Contributions of
René Descartes, Benjamin Franklin, and Louis Pasteur
Materialism, as a general philosophical term, considers matter as the sole basis
of reality and thus attempts to explain phenomena as the consequence of the
interaction of various types of matter. Applied to medicine, materialism implies
4 History of Medicine, Methods, and Psychotherapy
that any bodily state, including most importantly illness, has a physical substrate.
Specificity, which is a corollary of materialism, refers to the manner in which
treatments render their effects. A treatment is said to be a specific treatment if
the components of the treatment address illness by altering those physiochemi-
cal aspects of the body that were responsible for the illness. Generally, specificity
in medicine relies on demonstrable alterations of the physiochemical process
responsible for the disease and either a removal of the disease (i.e., cure) or a
reduction in the severity of the disorder above what could be obtained through
effects created by the mind by such factors as hope, expectation, and condition-
ing. Although materialism, as a philosophy, has been around since the ancient
Greeks, establishing specificity in medicine depended on the development of
the sciences of anatomy and physiology to explain the causes of disease and
on the development of research design and statistical methods to appropriately
test the effects of treatments.
Before Benjamin Franklin and Louis Pasteur could make their contributions
to modern medicine, a philosophical issue needed to be resolved. For most of
human history, there was no distinction between physical and mental disorders;
indeed the sciences of anatomy and physiology were not sufficient to claim
that physical disorders resided in the body and mental disorders in the mind.
The imbalances in the Pythagorean humors were sufficient to explain physi-
cal and mental disorders and there was no corroboration or refutation of the
conjecture relative to these disorders possible. (Of course, the idea of refutation
of hypotheses empirically was not yet developed, so it wasn’t simply a lack of
knowledge of anatomy and physiology.) If medicine were to find the material
bases of disorders, it would be in the arena of physical disorders and the dis-
tinction between physical and mental disorders was consequently necessary. It
was René Descartes, in the early seventeenth century, who made the distinction
between mind and body, although it was not his purpose to be at the service of
the development of medicine, as he was interested in the mind in an ontologi-
cal sense. Nevertheless, the distinction placed anatomy and physiology, which
were now subject to observation, on an empirical track; the mind remained in
the metaphysical realm and, in a manner of speaking, became the province of
psychology. Of course, as a note, there is much interest in the past few decades
on the interaction of mind and body, and advances in the neurosciences are
eliminating, according to some perspectives, the notion of the mind as distinct
from the body—rather the mind is what the brain does (Miller, 1996).
As science and the scientific method were evolving in Europe in the Carte-
sian context, it became apparent that most of the substances in the pharma-
copoeias were not effective. Indeed, only a very few seemed to be effective for
particular diseases (e.g., foxglove for congestive heart conditions and cinchona
bark for malaria) (Shapiro & Shapiro, 1997b). In 1785 the term placebo entered
the medical lexicon and was applied to treatments that were known to be inef-
fective physiochemically but satisfied the patient’s desire to be treated (Shapiro
& Shapiro, 1997b). The term, according to Walach (2003), originated from
History of Medicine, Methods, and Psychotherapy 5
the Latin psalm verse, “Placebo Domino in regione vivorum” (“I shall please
the Lord in the land of the living”), which was sung in the Middle Ages as a
prayer at the deathbed. Because others were often paid to do the singing, the
term placebo became associated with a “nearly fraudulent replacement of the
real” (Walach, 2003, p. 178). As will become apparent in subsequent discus-
sions, placebo and the effects that are derived from them are deeply imbedded
in several controversies in medicine and in psychotherapy; from the perspective
of this volume, an understanding of the placebo effect is critical to an under-
standing of psychotherapy. Nevertheless, the term placebo, from its origin, has
retained a tainted connotation—administration of a substance simply to please
the patient became repugnant and claims that a “placebo” was curative would
risk being labeled a charlatan, as Franz Anton Mesmer was soon to find out.
Contemporaneous to the development of the notion of the placebo, the
Parisian physician Mesmer had a lucrative medical practice. It was populated
with the elite of Paris but was also controversial. In his dissertation, Mesmer
(1766/1980) claimed that some illnesses were caused by the blockage of the
normal flow of an invisible universal fluid, which he called animal magnetism.
The physician could restore health by removing the blocks, and after further
“research,” Mesmer found that he could “magnetize” objects with animal
magnetism and these could be used to cure his clients (Buranelli, 1975; Gallo &
Finger, 2000; Gauld, 1992; Pattie, 1994). The success of this treatment was well
documented and led to its immense popularity in the late eighteenth century.
Mesmer, already caught up in several controversies, came under intense
scrutiny. Medicine, wanting to disavow practices that were unscientific, found
Mesmer’s cures uncomfortable. Responding to these forces, King Louis XVI of
France established in 1784 a Royal Commission, chaired by Benjamin Frank-
lin, to investigate mesmerism (Gould, 1991). Some of the experiments designed
by the commission involved patients being split into two groups, with one group
coming into contact with “magnetized” objects and the other group coming
into contact with what they believed were “magnetized” objects (i.e., according
to modern terminology, a placebo). Care was taken to ensure that the patients
did not know whether they were receiving a magnetized object or not, creating
one of the first, if the not the first, rigorous blinding in a study (here, a single
blind). This design enabled the Royal Commission to demonstrate, as there
were no differences in the cures produced by the two groups, that Mesmer’s
cures did not occur through treatment-specific ingredients.
The noted natural historian Stephen Jay Gould (1989) heralded the testing
and discrediting of Mesmer as one of the earliest and exemplary demonstra-
tions of the use of the scientific method to expose pseudoscience and charla-
tanism. However, imbedded in the Mesmer story are two points that should
not be lost in this progressive story. First, Mesmer’s treatments were effective,
as noted by the Royal Commission—benefits to patients were observable. Sec-
ond, Mesmer’s theories of illness and treatment were grounded in the best
science available, namely the theories proposed by Sir Isaac Newton, who only
6 History of Medicine, Methods, and Psychotherapy
a century earlier had crossed the threshold from a fascination with the occult
to the origins of mechanics and the advancement of mathematics (Gleick,
2003). Thus Mesmer was discredited not based on treatment effectiveness or
theoretical cogency but on the observation that the proposed mechanism of
illness remediation was questionable. This is a standard that mental health
treatments, pharmacological as well as psychological, will have, as we will see,
a difficult time satisfying. Of course, Mesmer’s exposure as a charlatan was a
conspicuous event that furthered medicine as a profession.
The third seminal person in the development of modern medicine was
Louis Pasteur, the father of germ theory (actually, paternity questions could
be raised by Robert Koch, who has a legitimate claim of fatherhood). Pasteur
exhibited the optimal blend of theory and experiment to, as the philosopher
Ernest Renan noted, “interrogate nature” until certain proofs of conjectures
were accomplished. The unifying theme, if one could characterize discoveries
across a vast array of areas as a theme, of his work is that Pasteur was able to
make inferences about the existence and characteristics of entities too small to
be observed directly. The story of how he made fermentation alive provides an
important anecdote about the inevitable interactions between epistemological
and ontological contributions (Latour, 1999).
In the 1850s, chemistry, having thrown off the vestiges of alchemy and find-
ing itself the preeminent field of science, sought chemical explanations for
most natural phenomena, including biological processes. The canonical view
at the time was that fermentation was the decay of sugars into alcohol by a
catalytic but unobserved “disintegrating disturbance,” which could be trans-
ferred from one batch of fermenting solution to another. Unfortunately for
alcohol producers, the process was unreliable and the chemical explanation
provided had little pragmatic value. Based on his prior work in crystallography
of organic substances, keen observation, and systematic experimentation, Pas-
teur hypothesized that living microorganisms were responsible for fermenta-
tion, rather than being created spontaneously as a result of the process. This
discovery led to other conclusions, including the conjecture that disease was
caused by microorganisms, which constituted the origins of the germ theory of
disease. The pairing of theory and experimentation resulted in medical prac-
tices with demonstrable benefits—vaccines using compromised organisms,
sterilization of medical environments, and sterilization of foods by heat (i.e.,
“pasteurization”).
Two aspects of the Pasteur story are critical, one quite obvious in retro-
spect and the other illustrative of more subtle implications for the philosophy
of science. Materialism applied to medicine requires physical explanations
for illness—the germ theory was exactly “what the doctor ordered.” Not only
could disease be cured or prevented, but the underlying mechanism could be
explicated in a demonstrable manner. To be sure, there was no lack of hypoth-
esized mechanisms prior to Pasteur. It was his beautifully constructed and in
the end inconvertible demonstration of how microorganisms caused disease
History of Medicine, Methods, and Psychotherapy 7
and the developments that followed as a result that changed the status of the
explanations.
From today’s perspective, the ontological nature of disease was clearly advanced
as a result of Pasteur’s work on fermentation and the idea of spontaneous gen-
eration seems absurd (see Latour, 1999), which brings us to the second point
relative to Pasteur. In 1864 the epistemological battle was just beginning to be
fought. At the time chemical decomposition was accepted as the explanation for
fermentation; the microbes observed were due to spontaneous generation as a
result rather than the cause of fermentation. Proponents of a microorganismic
explanation for fermentation, and there were some, were considered lunatics,
in the way we now think of Mesmer. The organisms were there, in retrospect,
and not there, in a contemporaneous way. In the 1860s, the disintegrating distur-
bance that catalyzed fermentation and the microbes that caused fermentation
both were unobservable. Pasteur not only ingeniously designed the experiments
so that the organisms could make themselves known and constructed the theory
to provide the cogency of the experimental results, but he rhetorically, in papers
and presentations, convinced the scientific world of the merit of his explana-
tion—the latter as difficult as the former. In a sense, Pasteur and the microor-
ganisms conspired together; neither one alone could have spawned the germ
explanation of disease (Latour, 1999).
What constitutes knowledge in a given field depends, in part, on the people
who conduct the research, create the theories, and influence the scientific com-
munity, particularly in the social sciences. Knowledge at any given time, as we
will argue in this volume, is tenuous—the nature of psychotherapy makes itself
known in response to our inquiries, but the nature of those inquiries shapes
what we accept as knowledge. We, as researchers, clinicians, and policymakers,
influence what is said to be knowledge. Descartes, Franklin, and Pasteur played
critical roles in developing, along with others in this the critical period of the
nineteenth century, the components that were necessary to form the model of
modern medicine.
The Medical Model
The Medical Model, undergirded by materialism and specificity and existing
within anatomy, physiology, microbiology, and other biological sciences, is, for
our purposes, composed of five components.
Illness or Disease
The first component is an illness or disease. The patient reports to the physi-
cian with signs and symptoms, which, along with the history, examination, and
laboratory tests, leads to a determination first whether the patient’s condition
is abnormal (i.e., deviates from what is considered normal human biological
functioning) and second, if abnormality exists, a diagnosis. Some interventions
8 History of Medicine, Methods, and Psychotherapy
are designed to prevent illness (e.g., vaccines); such preventative interventions
generally conform to the Medical Model as well.
Biological Explanation
The second component, emanating from the materialistic stance of medicine,
is that there is a biological explanation for the illness or disorder. For instance,
influenza is caused by a virus, which invades cells in the nose, throat, and lungs
of humans, where it replicates and mutates. Of course, the explanation progres-
sively becomes more sophisticated as science illuminates the process. Not infre-
quently, an explanation will turn out to be false and will be supplanted with a
better alternative, as was the progression of the explanation of peptic ulcers from
excess acid due to stress or spicy diet to the presence of the bacterium H. pylori.
Of course, the materialistic stance of medicine dictates that the explanation be
biological—something related to the anatomy or physiology of the body.
Mechanism of Change
The third component of the Medical Model is that the basis for treatment be
established at the level of the biological system causing the disease and a con-
jecture about how changing an aspect of the system will eliminate the disease
or mitigate the severity or duration of the illness. When the cause of pep-
tic ulcers was thought to be acids produced by the stomach due to stress or
diet, the mechanism of change involved neutralizing acids and changing diets,
whereas if an H. pylori infection were verified, the mechanism of change would
involve reducing the population of the bacteria with antibiotics.
Therapeutic Procedures
The presence of an explanation and the mechanism of change lead logically
to the design of a treatment, containing therapeutic procedures, which might
involve administration of a substance (i.e., a drug) or implementation of a proce-
dure (e.g., surgery). The explanation of excess acid due to stress (the explanation)
and the goal to reduce acid (mechanism of change) would suggest the adminis-
tration of a substance known to neutralize acid (i.e., an alkaline substance, such
as an antacid containing calcium carbonate). If an infection of H. pylori is veri-
fied, then the therapeutic ingredient would be an antibiotic. Medical treatments
generally require that the therapeutic procedures be consistent with the explana-
tion for the illness, disease, or disorder and the mechanism of change.
Specificity
Mesmer’s treatments based on animal magnetism conformed to the first four
components of the Medical Model: patients presented with signs and symptoms
History of Medicine, Methods, and Psychotherapy 9
of illness, there was a biological explanation for the disorder, a hypothesized
mechanism of change existed, and a particular therapeutic procedure was
followed. Mesmer’s treatment, however, failed the specificity test. Specific-
ity in the context of medicine, as already discussed, implies that the compo-
nents of the treatment are remedial through alterations of physiochemical
aspects of the body that were responsible for the illness. Antibiotics for peptic
ulcers are specific to the degree to which they work by killing the bacteria rather
than through other means, including but not limited to hope, expectation, or
conditioning. Mesmer’s cures were not specific because animal magnetism was
shown not to be responsible for the benefits of his treatments.
In medicine, specificity is established in two primary ways. First, the treat-
ment can be shown to be more effective than a placebo treatment, thus ruling
out incidental causes related to the context of the treatment. For example, with
adequate controls, if a pill is superior to a placebo, then presumably it is for
reasons unrelated to whether the patient expects the pill to be effective or is
conditioned to respond to pills in general (e.g., see Hentschel, Brandstätter,
Dragosics, Hirschl, Nemec, et al., 1993). The development of the randomized
placebo control group will be reviewed and the logic of the design discussed in
the following section.
The second means to establish specificity is to establish that the medical
treatment operates through its intended mechanism. Administration of antibi-
otics leads to a decrease in H. Pylori, which subsequently leads to healing of the
ulcer, lending support for the explanation and the mechanism of change and
thus lending support for specificity—the antibiotic works through the intended
mechanism (see Hentschel et al., 1993). Indeed, much of Pasteur’s research
was focused on explanation, mechanism, and specificity. Studies of the mecha-
nisms of disease and the effects of treatment on the intervening biological sys-
tems often precede the clinical trials that are used to establish efficacy. There
are, however, salient instances in which a drug is known to be effective but for
unknown reasons. Acetylsalicylic acid (commonly known as aspirin) was used as
an analgesic, anti-inflammatory, and antipyretic (fever reducer) before its bio-
logical mechanisms were understood.
Adaptation of the Medical Model to psychotherapy is a controversial proj-
ect, which in many ways is the subject of this volume. As we will see, the devel-
opment of psychotherapy as a treatment for mental disorders is entwined with
the development of medicine. Medicine, of course, is the predominant force
and psychotherapy is subordinate.
Evidence-Based Medicine
The development of the Medical Model and the genesis of “modern medi-
cine” without much argument resulted in positive health outcomes, including
cures of many diseases and prevention of others. To wit, small pox has been
eradicated, poliomyelitis is prevented by vaccine, deaths due to post-surgical
10 History of Medicine, Methods, and Psychotherapy
infection are rare, and antibiotics are able to treat most bacterial infections.
Nevertheless, materialism and specificity as the ontological bases of medicine
and the progress made by discrediting charlatans and creating a laboratory
science of microbiology did not directly translate into implementation of treat-
ments that resulted in optimal outcomes for patients. One hundred twenty-five
years after Mesmer’s treatments were subjected to examination and more than
50 years after Pasteur debunked spontaneous generation and established the
germ theory of disease, medicine clung to many “primitive” practices. Prior to
the First World War and the influenza epidemic of 1918, the typical medical
school in the United States was unaffiliated with a college or university, was
staffed by part-time faculty whose salaries were paid directly from student fees,
was populated by students who had not taken any science courses, let alone
attended college, and depended on a curriculum in which students never exam-
ined or treated patients and infrequently used laboratory equipment (Barry,
2004). In 1910, the Flexner report changed the nature of medical education in
the United States and Canada, and in a relatively short period of time medical
education became rigorous, competitive, and scientific. Nevertheless, little was
known about the efficacy of many medications and procedures—indeed, it
was not until the 1950s that the randomized placebo control group design was
developed, and it was not until 1980 that the Food and Drug Administration
(FDA) required such designs be used to approve drugs in the United States, as
discussed in the next section.
Numerous examples can be found to document how medical practice has
ignored accumulating evidence. For our purposes, the case of streptokinase,
an enzyme that dissolves clots, as a treatment for acute myocardial infarction is
illustrative (Hunt, 1997). Clinical trials of streptokinase began as early as 1959,
but the results, due to small sample sizes, were inconclusive, as some found a
significantly better outcome than the placebo control group while others did
not. However, as early as 1969 there was sufficient evidence, had the trials been
meta-analyzed, to conclude that this intervention was effective. Iain Chalmers,
an early advocate of meta-analyses as a means to make conclusions that could
be translated into medical practice, made the following observation:
Streptokinase was the classic example. The meta-analyses showed clearly
that the effect on mortality was statistically significant, but the experts in
cardiology and the textbook authors whose opinions dominated the field
weren’t even beginning to recommend it until the late 1980s, and then
only little by little.
(quoted in Hunt, 1997, p. 87)
From the time the evidence was persuasive to the time streptokinase was
accepted as standard practice following FDA approval in 1987, it is esti-
mated that tens of thousands of patients died because they were not admin-
istered streptokinase.
History of Medicine, Methods, and Psychotherapy 11
The streptokinase example is one of many that led to the initiation of a
movement to ensure that research evidence was translated into practice. This
movement, called evidence-based medicine, initiated in the United Kingdom and
Canada, emphasizes systematic and analytic reviews of evidence and the use
of that evidence by clinicians. In 2001, the Institute of Medicine in the United
States adopted the following definition, following closely from Sackett, Straus,
Richardson, Rosenberg, and Haynes (2000): “Evidence-based practice is the
integration of best research evidence with clinical expertise and patient val-
ues” (p. 147). This definition has been described as a “three legged stool,” in
that the use of evidence (first leg) is to be balanced with the expertise of the
clinician (second leg) and characteristics and context of the patient (third leg).
Nevertheless, an examination of the seminal book on evidence-based medi-
cine, Evidence-based Medicine: How to Practice and Teach EBM (Sackett et al., 2000)
reveals that the focus is on evidence related to the quality of diagnostic tests and
effectiveness of treatments.
Intimately tied to the evolution of modern medicine is the development
of methods that could establish specificity, most importantly the randomized
double-blind placebo control group design. Consequently, our story now turns
to the history of this design.
Randomized Designs as the “Gold Standard”
Randomized designs were needed by medicine to discriminate effects due to
the purported active ingredients from those due to the “mind,” such as hope,
expectation, and relationship with the administrator of the substance or pro-
cedure. What is now known as the double-blind randomized placebo control
group design, which is the “gold standard” for FDA approval of drugs, is a rela-
tively recent invention. The history of the development of this design is critical
to understanding the current status of psychotherapy as well as medicine and
will reveal some important aspects of the therapeutic process that were omitted.
The Development of Randomization and
Comparison Designs
Three strands, according to Danziger (1990), were intertwined to develop
the notion of control group designs. The first strand emanated from Wilhelm
Wundt, who established experimentation in psychology. In Wundt’s laboratory,
he and his students were observers, as they conceived of themselves as trained sci-
entists who could report on and interpret aspects of the mind, much in the way
that a physicist would interpret the photographic trace of a particle in a cloud
chamber. Wundt and the students would design experimental protocols and
manipulate various stimuli to record the effects, which were based on reports
of internal perception (i.e., one type of introspection). The stimulus/response
contiguity was the predominant model in experimental physiology at the time.
12 History of Medicine, Methods, and Psychotherapy
The responses reported by the Wundt’s observers were typically “judgments
of size, intensity, and duration of physical stimuli, supplemented at times by
judgments of their simultaneity and succession” (Danziger, 1990, p. 35) and
were used to derive general laws, mainly of sensation and perception.
At around the time of Wundt’s laboratory experiments, the notion of “sub-
jecting” lay individuals to various conditions can be traced to experimental
studies of hypnosis in France and constitutes the second strand. These studies
differed in an important way from Wundt’s—the French scientists were the
experimenters and the patients were subjects, a clear departure from the Wun-
dtian tradition in which the scientists were subjected to experiments and also
were observers (and authors of the research reports). That is, in the French
context the role of the experimenter (the observer) and the role of the subject
were separated. The change in role allowed the observation of classes of sub-
jects who were not able to report internal states (e.g., children) and/or whose
reports were suspect (e.g., people with mental illnesses). The paradigm, largely
steeped in the French medical context, inevitably evolved into clinical research
in that “healthy subjects” were compared to abnormal subjects with the goal
of discovering essential differences between the two classes (Danziger, 1990).
Nevertheless, in these experiments, the experimenter, a physician, had a profes-
sional relationship with the patients. It should be noted, for historical accuracy,
sporadic examples of comparisons of various samples existed before the French
physician studies (e.g., James Lind’s experiments with scurvy in the eighteenth
century), but the idea of “subjecting” participants to treatments appears to be
derived from this French tradition.
The third strand involved “applicants,” rather than observers or subjects,
and the “applicants” were not abnormal, at least at the origin of this strand.
The applicants were volunteers who paid to have their “mental faculties” tested
by Francis Galton in England, many at the International Health Exhibition
in London in 1884. During this period, phrenology was widely accepted as
a means of assessing mental abilities and there was keen interest in knowing
where one stood in relation to others. To accomplish such comparisons, Galton
and other British social statisticians, such as Karl Pearson, needed to quantify
mental ability and to locate that quantity in a distribution of scores—the impor-
tant determination was how one’s score deviated from the average (Danziger,
1990; Desrosières, 1998). In this approach, the relationship between investiga-
tor and subject was minimal: “For the Galtonian investigator the individual sub-
ject was ultimately ‘a statistic’” (Danziger, 1990, p. 58). The British statistical
approach contributed the critical concepts of measurement of unobservable
characteristics (in this case, mental abilities) and statistical distributions of such
characteristics, critical components of analysis of the observation in random-
ized control group designs. These contributions were made in the context that
led to what was considered normal or above average mental abilities and thus
logically to a class of people who were mentally defective (i.e., those who were
not normal or above average). Unfortunately, this led as well to calibration of
History of Medicine, Methods, and Psychotherapy 13
“genetic worth,” which using the principles of Charles Darwin’s (Galton’s cousin)
theory of evolution, spawned the field of eugenics (see Desrosières, 1998).
Wundt introduced laboratory methods in psychology and attempted to
extract general rules. The French researchers devised designs in which the
experimenter subjected research participants to various conditions and com-
pared abnormal to normal persons. The British social statisticians provided
the statistical theory related to deviations from the mean. These were all criti-
cal components of clinical trials in medicine and psychotherapy, but the miss-
ing component to this mix was randomization. The impetus for that critical
component came, in part, from the desire to provide pragmatic knowledge
to various consumer groups. Academic psychologists deemed education an
apt context to demonstrate the utility of their nascent discipline. In the early
1920s, the treatment group methodology was “being sold to American school
superintendents as the ‘control experiment’ and touted as a key element in
comparing the ‘efficiency’ of various administrative measures” (Danziger,
1990, p. 114). Shortly thereafter, McCall (1923) published How to Experiment
in Education, which introduced control group experimentation in education
and discussed the notion of randomization. At about the same time, Sir Ron-
ald Fisher took a position at an agricultural station where he developed the
analysis of variance and various other procedures for comparing crop yields
(Gehan & Lemak, 1994). Fisher’s work in randomized experimental designs
and the analysis of data derived from such designs was absolutely stunning—
arguably the design and analysis of every clinical trial in medicine, psychol-
ogy, and education is based on methods developed by Fisher (Danziger, 1990;
Shapiro & Shapiro, 1997b) or derived from his work. Fisher’s publications,
most prominently The Design of Experiments, which appeared in 1935, became
particularly useful to medical researchers eager to show the efficacy of various
medications, although one additional component, the placebo control, was
needed (Gehan & Lemak, 1994).
Introduction of Placebo Controls to Rule
Out Nuisance Variables
The goal of modern medicine was to establish that the benefits of any medical
treatment were due to the physiochemical properties of the medication and not
to the patient’s expectations, hopes, or other psychological processes, thereby
establishing the specificity of the purported active ingredients of the medica-
tion. To rule out threats due to these psychological factors, researchers in the
late 1930s began to use double-blind placebo studies in the United States and
the United Kingdom, but the method did not take root, apparently because
placebo carried a negative connotation (Gehan & Lemak, 1994; Shapiro &
Shapiro, 1997a, b). Gradually however, the acceptance of the randomized
double-blind placebo design spread. Harry Gold, a pharmacologist and one of
those given credit for the development of the placebo control group design in
14 History of Medicine, Methods, and Psychotherapy
the United States, participated in several conferences at Cornell University on
the subject in the late 1940s and early 1950s; he became the first professor of
clinical pharmacology, a new discipline. As noted by Shapiro and Shapiro (1997b):
Gold advocated a comparison between “an allegedly potent agent and a
blank of such physical properties as to render a distinction between the
two impossible except through some pharmacologic potency which may
exist. . . . [the recommended] double-blind procedure which calls for an
investigation in which neither the patient nor the doctor is aware of the
identify of the two agents until the results are in and analyzed. This is
imperative to avoid the influence of subconscious bias . . .” (Gold, 1954,
p. 724). The statement by Gold culminated twenty years of pioneering
study of methods with which to reliably and validly evaluate the effective-
ness of new drugs.
(p. 148)
By 1980, the FDA required that evidence for the effectiveness of a drug be
obtained from randomized double-blind placebo trials—historically speaking,
a relatively recent development (see Figure 1.1 ).
The importance of the randomized double-blind placebo control group
design methodologically and conceptually should not be underestimated. It
took more than 300 years from Descartes’ dualism of mind and body and
nearly 200 years from the time that Mesmer was discredited on specificity
claims to the institutionalization of a design that could rule out psychological
threats to the establishment of the specific effects of substances on the body
(see Figure 1.1 ).
Before leaving the short history of experimental designs, it is worth reiterat-
ing that there are two critical features of the randomized double-blind placebo
control group design. The first is that the placebo administered as a control
for psychological factors should be indistinguishable from the treatment in all
respects. For trials of medications, the pharmaceutical industry manufactures
placebos that are identical to the purportedly active medications in taste, shape,
color, and form. One of the problems, as will be discussed in later chapters, is
that subjects will try to guess which condition they are in and will use any cues
available (e.g., the presence or absence of side effects).
The second critical feature of the randomized double-blind placebo control
group is related to blinding. In actuality, the “double-blind” moniker refers to a
triple-blind: the administrator of the intervention, the patient, and the evalua-
tor, throughout the study, are ignorant of which treatment (medication or pla-
cebo) the patient is receiving. Any deviation from the blind could result in bias,
either directly (e.g., by an evaluator who might unconsciously score a protocol
to favor the medication) or through cues provided to the patient (e.g., greater
enthusiasm when delivering the medication than when delivering the placebo).
Issues in blinding will be discussed in subsequent chapters. 1
Figure 1.1
Timeline of important events in psychotherapy.
16 History of Medicine, Methods, and Psychotherapy
Psychotherapy emerged in the context of the development of modern medi-
cine and utilized in part the randomized design to legitimate its standing. We
now turn to the third history.
The Emergence of Psychotherapy
as a Healing Practice
The Origins of Talk Therapy in the United States
In the short history of modern medicine, little mention was made of mental
disorders. In the late nineteenth century, medicine was attempting to be seen
as a legitimate profession based on scientific principles, and as discussed previ-
ously, medicine emphasized physiochemical (i.e., somatic) processes. Medicine’s
attitude toward mental health problems was one of psychophysical parallel-
ism: mental states corresponded with physical states and it was hypothesized
that mental disorders were caused by some (unknown) physiochemical process
(Caplan, 1998). Of course, most disorders, mental or physical, at the time had
unknown causes; they were classified as functional (cause unknown) as opposed to
structural (i.e., cause known). Attempts were made to discover the physiochemical
causes of mental disorders and, for the most part, psychosocial causes and any
type of mental therapeutics (e.g., talk therapy) were assiduously avoided.
According to Caplan (1998), several events in the United States conspired
to challenge the emphasis on physiochemical explanations. First, the train as
a means of transportation emerged. Trains, of course, differed in many ways
from previous forms of transportation, but for purposes of the development of
psychotherapy, the important aspect was that trains, when things went awry,
created catastrophic collisions, which produced a multitude of various injuries.
A frequent complaint of those in the collisions involved a diffuse constella-
tion of symptoms, which usually included back pain, and led to the diagnosis
of “railway spine.” What was troubling for medicine was that witnesses on
the platforms near the collisions reported many of the same symptoms even
though they had not been involved in the physical trauma, a phenomenon that
cast doubt on a physiochemical cause of reported symptoms and introduced
the notion that the mind has a role in symptoms.
The second perspicuous precursor of psychotherapy was related to the dis-
order neurasthenia, which was characterized by fatigue, anxiety, headache,
impotence, neuralgia, and depression, and which became a prevalent disorder
in the United States. Although of unknown origin, it was hypothesized that it
was caused by a depletion of energy in the nervous system. Not surprisingly,
the treatments for neurasthenia varied dramatically, which interested some pre-
scient physicians:
How was it, certain physicians asked, that so many different modalities of
somatic therapies ranging from electricity and hydrotherapy to diet, rest,
History of Medicine, Methods, and Psychotherapy 17
nutrition, and medication could achieve identical results? Might they not
share a common ground? Deducing from the variegated experiences of a
wide array of somatic treatments, the Boston neurologist Morton Prince
declared, “I think if these treatments are carefully analyzed it will be found
that there is one factor common in them all, namely, the psychical element.”
(emphasis added, Caplan, 1998, p. 45)
Needless to say, the introduction of the psyche in medical circles was not well
received.
The third precursor to psychotherapy was development in various contexts
of “mind cures.” In the mid- to end of the nineteenth century, as medicine con-
centrated on the physiochemical, an increasing number of Americans turned
toward practices that healed through the mind, most popular of which were
Christian Science and the New Thought movement. Although these move-
ments may seem to be relics of the past or marginalized religious practices, they
were immensely popular (Caplan, 1998; Taylor, 1999). Christian Science, with
only about 9,000 members in 1890, had more than 50,000 by 1906 (Caplan),
which perhaps seems a modest number, but Christian Science was only one of
many institutions that claimed to physically heal through mind, faith, or spiri-
tuality. Cushman (1992) attributes the popularity of these movements to the
lack of a spiritual core in America and the desire to break free of the rigidity of
a Victorian society (see also Taylor, 1999).
At first, medicine deliberately dismissed these movements, for the most part,
as unscientific attempts to cure illnesses, whether physical or mental. Legiti-
macy lagged popularity, but gradually the involvement of American psycholo-
gists lent credibility to the idea of talk therapy as distinct from the religious
movements. The Boston School of Psychopathology, initiated in 1859, which
was an informal group of investigators, including the psychologists William
James and G. Stanley Hall as well as neurologists and psychiatrists, was to
become the epicenter of the new talk therapy. In 1906, the Emmanuel Move-
ment was initiated as a collaboration between physicians who recognized the
importance of the psyche and Christian ministers who recognized the moral
aspect to behavior; lectures and services were followed by the administration
of “therapy” to patients (Caplan, 1998; Taylor, 1999). The particularly threat-
ening aspect of the Emmanuel Movement for medicine was that patients were
being treated by psychical methods and often by non-physicians.
The manifestation of physical symptoms in the absence of physical cause,
the efficacy of various incompatible treatments for a prevalent disorder, and
the increasing popularity and legitimacy of talk therapies for physical and
mental disorders were problematic for the emerging modern medical profes-
sion. And thus a dilemma for medicine: reject the emerging psychotherapeu-
tics because it treated psychic disorders with non-medical means (viz., talk) or
absorb the lucrative professional practice of mental therapeutics. Interestingly,
talk therapies in America at this period had a connection with those interested
18 History of Medicine, Methods, and Psychotherapy
in Mesmer’s cures, which surely aroused the suspicions of physicians (Caplan,
1998; Cushman, 1992). Given the context, it is not surprising that medicine
resisted acknowledging mental factors in the etiology, pathology, and treatment
of mental disorders, and efforts were made to discredit mental therapeutics. On
the other hand, some psychologists, although not all by any means, were inter-
ested in such treatments and their mechanisms: “As early as 1894, [William]
James publicly assailed a proposal to proscribe the practice of mental healing,
‘What the real interest of medicine requires,’ James proclaimed, ‘is that mental
therapeutics should not be stamped out, but studied, and its laws ascertained’”
(Caplan, 1998, p. 63).
In the end, medicine could not allow patients to be treated outside of the
medical authority and it exerted its professional privilege to conduct psycho-
therapy. The sentiment was expressed by prominent physician John K. Mitch-
ell, “Most earnestly should we insist that the treatment of a patient, whether it be
surgical, medical, or psychic, should for the safety of the public, be in the hands
of a doctor” (Caplan, 1998, p. 142). What was missing was a cogent theory of
mental disorder and that would soon be provided.
Theoretical Orientations
Freud and the Origins of Psychodynamic Psychotherapy
When Sigmund Freud gave his lectures at Clark University in 1909, talk ther-
apy was established as a legitimate medical practice in the United States, but
he provided the missing theoretical coherence and all the better that it was
provided by a physician and in the medical context. Within six years, psycho-
analysis had become the predominant form of psychotherapy in the United
States: “Psychoanalysis appeared to be more proper and civilized than mind
cure, more scientific than Christian Science and positive thinking, and more
medical than advertising” (Cushman, 1992, p. 38).
Sigmund Freud, in his practice as a physician, became involved with the
treatment of hysterics. He proposed that a) hysteric symptoms were caused by
the repression of some traumatic event (real or imagined) in the unconscious;
b) the nature of the symptom was related to the event; and c) the symptom
could be relieved by insight into the relationship between the event and the
symptom. Moreover, from the beginning (as in his discussion of Anna O.), sex-
uality became central to the etiology of hysteria, with many symptoms associ-
ated with early sexual traumas. Freud experimented with various techniques
to retrieve repressed memories, including hydrotherapy, hypnosis, and direct
questioning, eventually promoting free association and dream analysis. From
these early origins of psychoanalysis, the components of the Medical Model
were apparent: a disorder (hysteria), a scientifically based explanation of the
disorder (repressed traumatic events), a mechanism of change (insight into
unconscious), and specific therapeutic actions (free association).
History of Medicine, Methods, and Psychotherapy 19
During his lifetime, Freud and his colleagues differed on various aspects
related to theory and therapeutic action, creating irreconcilable rifts with such
luminaries as Joseph Breuer, Alfred Adler, and Carl Jung, the latter two of
whom were expelled from Freud’s Vienna Psychoanalytic Society. The Medi-
cal Model is characterized by insistence on the correct explanation of a disor-
der and adoption of the concomitant therapeutic actions that are responsible
for the patient benefits. Freud insisted that his theory was correct and that his
treatments were specific and supported by scientific evidence. Although from
a current vantage point, the empirical bases of Freudian psychoanalysis and
competing systems (e.g., Adler’s individual psychology or Jung’s analytic psy-
chology) seem to be tenuous, at best, there are claims that neuroscience has
corroborated many psychodynamic constructs and theory (e.g., Westen, 1998).
Regardless of the debates about the scientific merit of psychodynamic con-
cepts, it should be realized that Freud’s complex theories were introduced prior
to Flexner’s report and the reformation of medicine that resulted; that is, the
substance and bases for Freud’s claims were suitable for the period in which
they occurred.
One critical point for our history of psychotherapy is related to the degree
to which psychotherapy for the first half of the twentieth century was the prov-
ince of medicine. As we have seen, psychotherapy was already incorporated
into medicine at the turn of the century and Freud, a physician, provided an
explanation acceptable to the medical profession. Moreover, admittance to psy-
choanalytic institutes and the practice of psychoanalysis was limited primarily
to physicians, further defining psychotherapy as a medical practice. Interest-
ingly, Freud himself trained lay (i.e., non-physician) analysts, the most notable
of whom was Theodore Reik, who was charged with the crime of practic-
ing medicine without a license. Although Reik was acquitted, upon Freud’s
death, access to the psychoanalytic institutes for non-physicians, including
psychologists, was further restricted (VandenBos, Cummings, & DeLeon,
1992). As noted by Jerome Frank (1992), until mid-century, in the research
context particularly, “The division of labor by discipline was unquestioned:
Psychologists did intelligence testing and assessment of personality, usually
the Rorschach test; social workers did the interviewing; and psychiatrists con-
ducted therapy” (p. 392).
An Alternative to Psychodynamic Approaches:
The Rise of Behaviorism
Behavioral psychology emerged as a parsimonious explanation of behavior
based on objective observations. Ivan Petrovich Pavlov’s work on classical con-
ditioning detailed, without resorting to complicated mentalistic constructs, how
animals acquired a conditioned response, how the conditioned response could
be extinguished (i.e., extinction), and how experimental neurosis could be
induced. John B. Watson and Rosalie Rayner’s “Little Albert Study” established
20 History of Medicine, Methods, and Psychotherapy
that a fear response could be conditioned by pairing a stimulus of fear (viz.,
loud noise) with a previously unconditioned stimulus (viz., a white rat that
Albert had played with without fear) so that the unconditioned stimulus (i.e.,
the rat) elicited the fear response (i.e., became a conditioned stimulus; Watson
& Rayner, 1920). Although Watson and Rayner did not attempt to alleviate
Albert’s fear, Mary Cover Jones (under the supervision of Watson) demon-
strated that the classical conditioning paradigm could be used to desensitize a
boy’s fear of rabbits by gradually increasing the proximity of the stimulus (i.e.,
the rabbit) to the boy in a pleasant state, which was established with Albert’s
favorite food.
A major impetus to behavioral therapy was provided by Joseph Wolpe’s
development of systematic desensitization. Wolpe, who like Freud was a medi-
cal doctor, became disenchanted with psychoanalysis as a method to treat
his patients. Based on the work of Pavlov, Watson, Rayner, and Jones, Wolpe
studied how eating, an incompatible response to fear, could be used to reduce
phobic reactions of cats, which he had previously conditioned. After studying
the work on progressive relaxation by physiologist Edmund Jacobson, Wolpe
recognized that the incompatibility of relaxation and anxiety could be used to
treat anxious patients. His technique, which was called systematic desensitiza-
tion, involves the creation of a hierarchy consisting of progressively anxiety-
provoking stimuli, which are then imagined by patients, under a relaxed state,
from least to most feared. His seminal book Psychotherapy by Reciprocal Inhibition,
in which he explicated how classical conditioning could be used as a psycho-
logical treatment, was published in 1958—at about the same time the medical
barrier was lowered and psychologists began to practice psychotherapy more
prevalently.
Although the explanation of anxiety offered by the psychoanalytic and clas-
sical conditioning paradigms differ dramatically, systematic desensitization has
many structural similarities to psychoanalysis. It is used to treat a disorder (pho-
bic anxiety), is based on an explanation for the disorder (classical condition-
ing), imbeds the mechanism of change within the explanation (desensitization),
and stipulates the therapeutic action necessary to effect the change (systematic
desensitization). So, although the psychoanalytic paradigm is saturated with
mentalistic constructs whereas the behavioral paradigm generally eschews
intervening mentalistic explanations, they are both systems that explain mal-
adaptive behavior and offer therapeutic protocols for reducing distress and
promoting more adaptive functioning. Proponents of one of the two systems
would claim that their explanations and protocols are superior to the other.
Indeed, Watson and Rayner (1920) were openly disdainful of any Freudian
explanation for Albert’s fears:
The Freudians twenty years from now, unless their hypotheses change,
when they come to analyze Albert’s fear of a seal skin coat—assuming
that he comes to analysis at that age—will probably tease from him the
History of Medicine, Methods, and Psychotherapy 21
recital of a dream which upon their analysis will show that Albert at three
years of age attempted to play with the pubic hair of the mother and was
scolded violently for it.
(p. 14)
The behaviorists claimed that they rejected the Medical Model. However,
it was the biological bases of mental illness that was antithetical to their theo-
retical position, as behaviorists at this period in time considered the child a
tabla rasa onto which experience writes and, therefore, problems of adult liv-
ing, including mental illness, are consequences of the learning history of the
individual. Nevertheless, the idea of treating a particular problem, say a simple
phobia, specifically with a particular treatment, say Wolpe’s systematic desensi-
tization, was critical to the behavioral paradigm.
As cognition gained a more prominent position in experimental psychol-
ogy, several psychotherapy theoreticians and researchers, some of whom
were trained as analysts or who were steeped in psychodynamic theory and
practice and others of whom came from a behavior or social learning per-
spective, developed models of cognitive therapy. It is debatable whether the
contributions of these luminaries, including Albert Ellis, Aaron Beck, Michael
Mahoney, and Donald Meichenbaum, represented a subsystem of behavior
therapy, which had become increasingly pragmatic and less tied to experimen-
tal paradigms (Fishman & Franks, 1992), or should be classified as a distinct
paradigm (Arnkoff & Glass, 1992). As we shall see, the issue of treatment
distinctiveness and evolution of treatment paradigms will reappear in several
prominent instances in this volume. For the purposes of this book, the term
cognitive-behavior therapy (CBT) will be used to refer to behavioral and cogni-
tive therapies, although in some instances CBT will be used to refer to a very
specific treatment modality. Indeed, as discussed in several places, the defini-
tion of CBT is ambiguous and there are disagreements about whether a par-
ticular treatment is or is not CBT.
Humanism as a Third Force
In the context of post–World War II modernism and attempts to make mean-
ing of life given the ravages of war and the Holocaust, psychotherapy devel-
oped a third force (after psychoanalysis and behavioral therapy) derived from
the humanistic philosophers (e.g., Kierkegaard, Husserl, and Heidegger).
Humanistic approaches have in common a) a phenomenological perspective
(i.e., therapy must involve understanding the client’s world); b) an assumption
that humans seek growth and actualization; c) a belief that humans are self-
determining; and d) a respect for every individual, regardless of their role or
actions (Rice & Greenberg, 1992). The best known of the original human-
istic therapies are person-centered therapy (Carl Rogers, as discussed e.g.,
in Client-Centered Therapy, 1951a), Gestalt Therapy (Frederick “Fritz” Perls),
22 History of Medicine, Methods, and Psychotherapy
and existential approaches (e.g., Rollo May and Victor Frankl). Humanistic
approaches emanated from distinctly non-medical origins and non-experimental
traditions, having roots more in philosophy than in science and medicine.
Status of Various Psychotherapies
Although dividing up the psychotherapy universe into three forces, psychody-
namic, cognitive-behavioral, and humanistic, is arbitrary, it is revealing to ask,
“What is the relative status of these forces?” Status, of course, is an ambiguous
term, but three sources of information relative to the question are available:
texts and other artifacts, psychotherapy practice, and research foci.
In a report on National Public Radio’s All Things Considered ( June 2, 2004) on
the treatment of a woman suffering social phobia, Alix Spiegel began by stating,
“Cognitive-behavioral therapy is the fastest growing and most rigorously stud-
ied form of psychotherapy. It is fast becoming what people in America mean
when they say they are getting therapy.” And it is not only the media that give
primacy to some treatments over others. The Oxford Textbook of Psychotherapy
(Gabbard, Beck, & Holmes, 2005), a comprehensive and voluminous treatment
of the subject, indicated that the editors “tried to ensure that the diverse psy-
chotherapeutic strategies were represented in a balanced way in each chapter,”
but the 534-page text discusses primarily cognitive-behavioral and psychody-
namic approaches—humanistic approaches and their developers were mostly
ignored (Wampold & Imel, 2006). As noted by Rice and Greenberg (1992),
“During the last two decades . . . the humanistic psychotherapy approaches
have become increasingly separated from mainstream theoretical psychology,
especially in North America” (p. 214).
The types of treatments delivered by therapists constitute another indicator of
the status of psychotherapies. Every 10 years, Norcross and colleagues survey
psychologists with regard to a number of practices, including type of treatment
provided (see Norcross & Karpiak, 2012; Norcross, Karpiak, & Santoro, 2005,
for the most recent surveys of clinical psychologists). The results of the survey
show a remarkable rise in the proportion of clinical psychologists who report
that their orientation was cognitive: in the 1960s and 1970, virtually no clinical
psychologist reported that they were cognitively oriented, whereas in the most
recent survey (viz., 2010), about one-third do so. If one combines cognitive with
behavioral, which has been steadily rising from 8 percent in 1960 to 15 percent in
2010, then 45 percent of clinical psychologists in the United States report that
their primary orientation is either cognitive or behavioral (Norcross et al., 2012).
On the other hand, the proportion that report a dynamic or eclectic/intregrative ori-
entation has decreased from 35 percent and 36 percent, respectively, in 1960 to
18 percent and 22 percent in 2010, respectively. All other orientations, includ-
ing Rogerian, humanistic, systems, and interpersonal, among others, were only
endorsed by 14 percent of the clinical psychologists responding in the 2010 sur-
vey. Of course, psychotherapy is not solely practiced by clinical psychologists,
History of Medicine, Methods, and Psychotherapy 23
but it appears, nevertheless, that not only have humanistic approaches been
abandoned (or perhaps have abandoned) mainstream theoretical psychology,
as Rice and Greenberg (1992) suggested, but psychotherapists (at least psy-
chologists) have abandoned these approaches as well.
The third source of information, which is derived from research foci, is dis-
cussed in the next section, where a history of developments in psychotherapy
research is presented. The conclusion here, however, is that cognitive behavioral
treatments clearly enjoy an elite status; however, many practicing therapists
indicate that they are delivering eclectic or integrative forms of psychotherapy.
Research Methods, Psychotherapy Efficacy, and
the Ascendancy of Treatments for Disorders
The need to demonstrate the efficacy of psychotherapy in general and vari-
ous treatments specifically shaped the development of psychotherapy. Research
methods played an important role in this development, as more advanced
methods were needed to demonstrate the effects of psychotherapy.
Research Methods Driven by a Need to
Demonstrate Efficacy
Research methods in psychotherapy have paralleled those of medicine. A clear
example is found with Freud, who considered himself a scientist but preferred
clinical findings of his treatments to results of statistical analyses of data. This
was not surprising given the state of such methods in the early twentieth cen-
tury. In the case method used by Freud, only trained psychoanalysts could be
“objective and impartial observers” to determine the outcomes of a specific
treatment. The case methods used by Freud and colleagues documented that
their treatments were remarkably successful but created much doubt by those
outside of the psychoanalytic community (Strupp & Howard, 1992). Indeed,
one of the continuing criticisms of psychoanalytic approaches has been the
lack of objective verification of outcomes.
The first direct observation of psychotherapy emanated from the humanistic
tradition, which is somewhat surprising given the phenomenological bent of
this school. While advocates of other approaches, particularly the psychoana-
lysts, were loathe to invade the sanctity of the interview room, in the 1940s
Carl Rogers and his group prepared transcripts of sessions from audio tapes,
a technology that was evolving at the time (Rice & Greenberg, 1992). From
this source material, Rogers and his research group generated hypotheses that
were to be tested by the evolving research methods being developed in educa-
tion and psychology (see, e.g., Rogers, 1951b). Rogers as well as researchers
at the Menninger Foundation and the University of Pennsylvania examined
whether psychotherapy resulted in changes in personality. Generally samples
were small, treatments were not well defined, disorders were not assessed and
24 History of Medicine, Methods, and Psychotherapy
codified, and outcomes were not well specified or operationalized (Goldfried &
Wolfe, 1996; Strupp & Howard, 1992).
Not long after Rogers first began his program of research, Hans Eysenck
published a series of articles and books (Eysenck, 1952, 1961, 1966) in which
he claimed that the rate of recovery of patients receiving psychotherapy was
equal to the rate of spontaneous remission, a damning indictment of the effects
of psychotherapy. The issue of psychotherapy effectiveness will be presented in
Chapter 4 , including an elaboration of the history related to the issue. It should
be noted here, however, that the term “psychotherapy” was used by Eysenck to
refer to psychodynamic, humanistic, and eclectic treatments; behavior therapy
in his view was distinct from these treatments as it was based on learning theory
(i.e., scientific principles). In Eysenck’s view, behavior therapy should be pre-
ferred to the alternatives treatments (see Wampold, 2013).
Eysenck’s claims generated much debate (see Chapter 4 ) and also instigated
increased rigor relative to the research designs employed to test the effects of
various psychotherapies (Wampold, 2013). The randomized design, developed
in the 1920s and 1930s, as well as the placebo control group design, devel-
oped in the 1950s, offered psychotherapy researchers designs that had the
potential to answer the question of whether psychotherapy was effective. In
1956, Rosenthal and Frank recommended the use of placebo-type controls in
psychotherapy research in order to establish the specificity as well as efficacy
of psychotherapy:
It may be possible to study the possible specific effects of any particular
form of therapy by the use of a matched control group participating in an
activity regarded therapeutically inert from the stand point of the theory
of the therapy being studied. That is, it would not be expected to produce
the effects predicted by the theory. The “placebo psychotherapy” in a sense
would be analogous to placebos in that it would be administered under
circumstances and by persons such that the patients would be expected to
be helped by it.
(pp. 299–300)
Although the use of placebo control groups in psychotherapy research is
problematic (see Chapter 8 ), historically Rosenthal and Frank’s recommenda-
tion was emblematic of psychotherapy’s close connection with medicine. Psy-
chotherapy was adopting models of research that were used by medicine to
demonstrate the effects of medications, thereby conceptualizing psychotherapy
as a medical treatment. This is a trend that has increased over the decades such
that beginning in the 1980s psychotherapy began to label its outcome research
as clinical trials as it sought to establish the viability of particular treatments for
particular disorders. The use of placebo-type control groups in psychotherapy
research was an attempt to show that psychotherapies, like drugs, were spe-
cific, which as we have seen, is a distinguishing feature of modern medicine.
History of Medicine, Methods, and Psychotherapy 25
Purportedly, the superiority of a particular psychotherapy to a placebo estab-
lishes the specificity of the treatment but also established the legitimacy of the
psychotherapy enterprise.
The next important development in psychotherapy research turns the tables,
as it originates in psychotherapy research (as well as in education) and was
“exported” to medicine. As will be seen more fully in Chapter 4 , one of the
issues that emanated from Eysenck’s claims was how the results of multiple
studies should be aggregated, as the conclusions were closely tied to the method,
particularly the manner in which studies were included and excluded and how
the results of the included studies were synthesized. Mary Lee Smith and Gene
Glass, in 1977, published a meta-analysis of all studies that compared a psy-
chotherapeutic approach to some type of control group, thereby demonstrat-
ing the utility of the method of meta-analysis, which will be described more
completely in Chapter 3 . Subsequently, meta-analysis has become the standard
method of aggregating research results in education, psychology, and medi-
cine. Importantly for psychotherapy, Smith and Glass (1977; Smith, Glass, &
Miller, 1980) found that psychotherapy was indeed efficacious, a conclusion
that will be examined fully in Chapter 4 .
An issue addressed by Smith and Glass, but one that was not resolved, was
so indigenous to modern medicine that it does not get addressed explicitly.
No one asks, “Does medicine work?” but rather, “Which treatment works best
for this particular disorder?” Post Smith and Glass, psychotherapy turned, à
la medicine, to identifying particular treatments that were effective for treat-
ing particular disorders (see Chapter 5 ). To address this issue, first treatments
were to be standardized, accomplished with treatment manuals, after which the
standardized treatments could be tested and compared.
Psychotherapy Treatment Manuals
A treatment manual contains “a definitive description of the principles and
techniques of [the] psychotherapy . . . [and] a clear statement of the operations
the therapist is supposed to perform (presenting each technique as concretely
as possible, as well as providing examples of each)” (Kiesler, 1994, p. 145).
The purpose of the treatment manual is to create standardization of treat-
ments, thereby reducing variability in the independent variable in clinical tri-
als, and to ensure that therapists deliver correctly the specific ingredients that
are characteristic of the theoretical approach. With regard to the latter point,
manuals enable “researchers to demonstrate the theoretically required pro-
cedural differences between alternative treatments in comparative outcome
studies” (Wilson, 1996, p. 295). Credit for the first treatment manual usually is
attributed to Beck, Rush, Shaw, and Emery (1979), who delineated cognitive-
behavioral treatment for depression. The proliferation of treatment manuals
since the Beck et al. manual in 1979 has been described as a “small revolu-
tion” (Luborsky & DeRubeis, 1984). Treatment manuals have become required
26 History of Medicine, Methods, and Psychotherapy
for the funding and publication of outcomes research in psychotherapy: “The
treatment manual requirement, imposed as a routine design demand, chis-
eled permanently into the edifice of psychotherapy efficacy research the basic
canon of standardization” (Kiesler, 1994, p. 145).
The treatment manual, as a research operation, is imbedded in the Medical
Model. The typical components of the manual, which include defining the tar-
get disorder, problem, or complaint; providing a theoretical basis for the disorder,
problem, or complaint; as well as the change mechanism; specifying the thera-
peutic actions that are consistent with the theory; and the belief that the specific
ingredients lead to efficacy, are identical to the components of the Medical Model.
Empirically Supported Treatments
The second development in psychotherapy research related to the issue of
treatments and disorders was the idea of “empirically supported treatments”
(ESTs). The emphasis in the 1990s on managed care in medicine and related
health areas, including mental health, created the need to standardize treat-
ments and provide evidence of efficacy. As diagnosis-related groups (DRGs),
which allowed fixed payment per diagnosis, became accepted in the medical
community, psychiatry responded with psychopharmacological treatments (i.e.,
drugs) for many mental disorders; the Medical Model in medicine was making
significant inroads in the treatment of mental disorders. A task force of Divi-
sion 12 (Clinical Psychology) of the American Psychological Association (APA)
reacted in a predictable way: “If clinical psychology is to survive in this heyday
of biological psychiatry, APA must act to emphasize the strength of what we
have to offer—a variety of psychotherapies of proven efficacy” (Task Force on
Promotion and Dissemination of Psychological Procedures, 1995, p. 3). Accord-
ingly, to identify treatments that would meet the criteria of being empirically
validated (the term originally used), the task force developed criteria, which if
satisfied by a treatment, would result in the treatment being included on a list
published by the task force. Although the criteria have evolved, they originated
from the criteria used by the FDA to approve drugs. The criteria essentially
stipulated that a treatment would be designated as empirically validated for
a particular disorder provided that at least two studies showed superiority to
groups that attempted to control for general effects and were administered to a
well-defined population of clients (including importantly the clients’ disorder,
problem, or complaint) using a treatment manual.
The first attempt to identify treatments that satisfied the criteria netted 18
well-established treatments (Task Force on Promotion and Dissemination of
Psychological Procedures, 1995). Revisions to the list were made subsequently
(Chambless et al., 1996; 1998) and included such treatments as cognitive behav-
ior therapy for panic disorder, exposure treatment for agoraphobia, behavior
therapy for depression, cognitive therapy for depression, interpersonal therapy
for depression, multicomponent cognitive-behavioral therapy for pain associated
History of Medicine, Methods, and Psychotherapy 27
with rheumatic disease, and behavioral marital therapy for marital discord. A
special issue of the Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology was devoted to a dis-
cussion of ESTs and the identification of empirically supported treatments for
adult mental disorders, child and adolescent disorders, health related disorders
(viz., smoking, chronic pain, cancer, and bulimia nervosa), and marital distress
(Baucom, Shoham, Mueser, Daiuto, & Stickle, 1998; Beutler, 1998; Borkovec
& Castonguay, 1998; Calhoun, Moras, Pilkonis, & Rehm, 1998; Chambless &
Hollon, 1998; Compas, Haaga, Keefe, Leitenberg, & Williams, 1998; Davison,
1998; DeRubeis & Crits-Christoph, 1998; Garfield, 1998; Kazdin & Weisz,
1998; Kendall, 1998).
With ESTs, psychotherapy had taken another step toward adopting the
Medical Model. First, the criteria clearly orient psychotherapy to disorder,
problem, or complaint: “We do not ask whether a treatment is efficacious; rather,
we ask whether it is efficacious for a specific problem” (Chambless & Hollon,
1998, p. 9). Although use of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM) as the
nosology for assigning disorders was not mandated, Chambless and Hollon
indicated the DSM has “a number of benefits” for determining ESTs; indeed
those who have reviewed research in order to identify ESTs typically use the
DSM to organize the review (e.g., DeRubeis & Crits-Christoph, 1998).
The requirement that only treatments administered with a manual are
certifiable as an EST further demonstrates a connection between ESTs and
the Medical Model because, as discussed above, manuals are intimately tied
to the Medical Model. The lists of empirically supported treatments were
dominated by behavioral and cognitive-behavioral treatments, with a few
exemplars of psychodynamic-derived treatments and no humanistic treat-
ments, which may reflect the fact that behavioral and cognitive-behavioral
treatments are easier to manualize than are humanistic or psychodynamic
treatments and fit more neatly into the clinical trial paradigm.
A third perspicuous aspect of the EST movement is the criteria, which were
patterned after the FDA drug-approval criteria that require that evidence is
needed relative to specificity as well as efficacy. According to the EST criteria,
specificity is established by demonstrating superiority to pill or psychological
placebo or by showing equivalence to an already-established treatment. Clearly,
specificity, a critical component in the Medical Model of psychotherapy, under-
girds the EST movement. 2 Indeed, the motivation to adopt a Medical Model
in order to bolster the status of psychotherapy was evident from the beginning:
We [The Task Force] believe establishing efficacy in contrast to a waiting
list control group is not sufficient. Relying on such evidence would leave
psychologists at a serious disadvantage vis-a-vis psychiatrists who can point
to numerous double-blind placebo trials to support the validity of their
interventions.
(Task Force on Promotion and Dissemination
of Psychological Procedures, 1995, p. 5)
References
Abramowitz, J. S. (1996). Variants of exposure and response prevention in the treatment of
obsessive-compulsive disorder: A meta-analysis. Behavior Therapy, 27, 583–600.
Abramowitz, J. S. (1997). Effectiveness of psychological and pharmacological treatments for
obsessive-compulsive disorder: A quantitative review. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 65, 44–52.
Adams, V. (1979, July 10). Consensus is reached: Psychotherapy works. New York Times, p.
C1.
Addis, M. E., Hatgis, C., Krasnow, A. D., Jacob, K., Bourne, L., & Mansfield, A. (2004).
Effectiveness of cognitive-behavioral treatment for panic disorder versus treatment as usual
in a managed care setting. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72 (4), 625–635.
doi: 10.1037/0022-006x.72.4.625
Ader, R. (1997). The role of conditioning in pharmacotherapy. In A.Harrington (Ed.), The
placebo effect: An interdisciplinary exploration (pp. 138–165). Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
Ahn, H., & Wampold, B. E. (2001). Where oh where are the specific ingredients? A meta-
analysis of component studies in counseling and psycotherapy. Journal of Counseling
Psychology, 48 (3), 251–257.
Albright, L., Kenny, D. A., & Malloy, T. E. (1988). Consensus in personality judgments at zero
acquaintance. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 55(3), 387–395.
Amanzio, M., Pollo, A., Maggi, G., & Benedetti, F. (2001). Response variability to anal-gesics:
A role for non-specific activation of endogenous opiods. Pain, 90, 205–211.
Ambady, N., & Rosenthal, R. (1993). Half a minute: Predicting teacher evaluations from thin
slices of nonverbal behavior and physical attractiveness. Journal of Personality & Social
Psychology, 64(3), 431–441.
Ambady, N., LaPlante, D., Nguen, T., Rosenthal, R., & Levinson, W. (2002). Surgeon’s tone
of voice: A clue to malpractice history. Surgery, 132, 5–9.
American Psychological Association, Office of Public Communications (1995, August).
Questions and answers about memories of childhood abuse. Retrieved from
www.apa.org/topics/memories.html
Anderson, A. S. (1988). Does psychotherapy make some clients worse? A reanalysis of the
evidence for treatment-induced deterioration. University of Memphis, Memphis, TN.
Anderson, T., Lunnen, K. M., & Ogles, B. M. (2010). Putting models and techniques in
context. In S. D.Miller, B. L.Duncan, M. A.Hubble & B. E.Wampold (Eds.), The heart and soul
of change (2nd ed., pp. 143–163). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Anderson, T., Ogles, B. M., Patterson, C. L., Lambert, M. J., & Vermeersch, D. A. (2009).
Therapist effects: facilitative interpersonal skills as a predictor of therapist success. Journal of
Clinical Psychology, 65(7), 755–768. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20583
Andrews, G., & Harvey, R. (1981). Does psychotherapy benefit neurotic patients?: A
reanalysis of the Smith, Glass, and Miller data. Archives of General Psychiatry, 38(11),
1203–1208.
Anholt, G. E., Kempe, P., de Haan, E., van Oppen, P., Cath, D. C., Smit, J. H., & van Balkom,
A.J.L.M. (2008). Cognitive versus behavior therapy: Processes of change in the treatment of
obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 77, 38–42.
APA Presidential Task Force on Evidence-Based Practice. (2006). Evidence-based practice
in psychology. American Psychologist, 61, 271–285.
Arch, J. J., Eifert, G. H., Davies, C., Vilardaga, J.C.P., Rose, R. D., & Craske, M. G. (2012).
Randomized clinical trial of cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) versus acceptance and
commitment therapy (ACT) for mixed anxiety disorders. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology (Supplemental). doi: 10.1037/a002831010.1037/a0028310.supp
Arch, J. J., Wolitzky-Taylor, K. B., Eifert, G. H., & Craske, M. G. (2012). Longitudinal
treatment mediation of traditional cognitive behavioral therapy and acceptance and
commitment therapy for anxiety disorders. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 50(7–8),
469–478. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2012.04.007
Arkowitz, H. (1992). Integrative theories of therapy. In D. K.Freedheim (Ed.), History of
psychotherapy: A century of change (pp. 261–303). Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Arnkoff, D. B., & Glass, C. R. (1992). Cognitive therapy and psychotherapy. In D.
K.Freedheim (Ed.), History of psychotherapy: A century of change (pp. 657–694).
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Arnow, B. A., Steidtmann, D., Blasey, C., Manber, R., Constantino, M. J., Klein, D. N., …
Kocsis, J. H. (2013). The relationship between the therapeutic alliance and treatment
outcome in two distinct psychotherapies for chronic depression. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 81(4), 627–638. doi: 10.1037/a0031530
Asimov, I. (1983). The roving mind. Amherst, NY: Promethius Books.
Association for Behavioral and Cognitive Therapies (ABCT). Retrieved April 21, 2014 from
www.abct.org/Information/?m=mInformation&fa=_WhatIsCBTpublic
Atkins, D. C., Steyvers, M., Imel, Z. E., & Smyth, P. (2014). Automatic evaluation of
psychotherapy language with quantitative linguistic models: An initial application to
Motivational Interviewing. Implementation Science. doi: 10.1037/a0036841
Baardseth, T. P., Goldberg, S. B., Pace, B. T., Wislocki, A. P., Frost, N. D., Siddiqui, J. R., …
Wampold, B. E. (2013). Cognitive-behavioral therapy versus other therapies: Redux. Clinical
Psychology Review, 33(3), 395–405. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr. 2013.01.004
Baker, T. B., McFall, R. M., & Shoham, V. (2008). Current status and future prospects of
clinical psychology: Toward a scientifically principled approach to mental and behavioral
health care. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 9(2), 67–103. doi: 10.1111/j.1539-
6053.2009.01036.x
Baldwin, S. A., Berkeljon, A., & Atkins, D. C. (2009). Rates of change in naturalistic
psychotherapy: Contrasting dose-effect and good-enough level models of change. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 77, 203–211.
Baldwin, S. A., & Imel, Z. E. (2013). Therapist effects: Findings and methods. In M. J.Lambert
(Ed.), Bergin and Garfield’s handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change (6th ed., pp.
258–297). New York: Wiley.
Baldwin, S. A., Murray, D. M., Shadish, W. R., Pals, S. L., Holland, J. M., Abramowtiz, J. S.,
… Watson, J. (2011). Intraclass correlation associated with therapists: Estimates and
applications in planning psychotherapy research. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 40, 15–33.
Baldwin, S. A., Wampold, B. E., & Imel, Z. E. (2007). Untangling the alliance-outcome
correlation: Exploring the relative importance of therapist and patient variability in the alliance.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75, 842–852.
Ball, S. A., Martino, S., Nich, C., Frankfort, T. L., Van Horn, D., Crits-Christoph, P., Woody, G.
E., Obert, J. L., Farentinos, C., & Carroll, K. M. (2007). Site matters: multisite randomized trial
of motivational enhancement therapy in community drug abuse clinics. Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology, 75, 556–567.
Bambling, M., King, R., Raue, P., Schweitzer, R., & Lambert, W. (2006). Clinical supervision:
Its influence on client-rated working alliance and client symptom reduction in the brief
treatment of major depression. Psychotherapy Research, 16(3), 317–331. doi:
10.1080/10503300500268524
Bandura, A. (1999). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. In R.
F.Baumeister (Ed.), The self in social psychology (pp. 285–298). New York: Psychology
Press.
Barber, J. P., Connolly, M. B., Crits-Christoph, P., Gladis, L., & Siqueland, L. (2009). Alliance
predicts patients’ outcome beyond in-treatment change in symptoms. Personality Disorders:
Theory, Research, and Treatment, 5(1), 80–89. doi: 10.1037/1949-2715.s.1.80
Barber, J. P., Gallop, R., Crits-Christoph, P., Frank, A., Thase, M. E., Weiss, R. D., &
Gibbons, M.B.C. (2006). The role of therapist adherence, therapist competence, and alliance
in predicting outcome of individual drug counseling: Results from the National Institute Drug
Abuse Collaborative Cocaine Treatment Study. Psychotherapy Research, 16, 229–240.
Barber, J. P., Muran, J. C., McCarthy, K. S., & Keefe, J. R. (2013). Research on dynamic
therapies. In M. J.Lambert (Ed.), Bergin and Garfield’s handbook of psychotherapy and
behavior change (6th ed., pp. 443–494). New York: Wiley.
Barcikowski, R. S. (1981). Statistical power with group mean as the unit of analysis. Journal
of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 6(3), 267–285.
Barker, S. L., Funk, S. C., & Houston, B. K. (1988). Psychological treatment versus
nonspecific factors: A meta-analysis of conditions that engender comparable expectations for
improvement. Clinical Psychology Review, 8, 579–594.
Barkham, M., Hardy, G. E., & Mellor-Clark, J. (2010). Developing and delivering practice-
based evidence: A guide for the psychological therapies. Chichester, UK: Wiley Blackwell.
Barlow, D. H. (2004). Psychological treatments. American Psychologist, 59, 869–878.
Barlow, D. H. (2010). The dodo bird–again–and again. The Behavior Therapist, 33(1), 15–16.
Barlow, D. H., Farchione, T. J., Fairholme, C. P., Ellard, K. K., Boisseau, C. L., Allen, L. B., &
Ehrenreich-May, J. (2011). Unified protocol for transdiagnostic treatment of emotional
disorders: Therapist guide. New York: Oxford University Press.
Barry, J. (2004). The great influenza: The story of the deadliest pandemic in history. New
York: Penguin.
Barth, J., Munder, T., Gerger. H., Nuesch, E., Trelle, S., (2013). Comparative Efficacy of
Seven Psychotherapeutic Interventions for Patients with Depression: A Network Meta-
Analysis. PLoS Medicine, 10(5): e1001454. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.1001454
Baskin, T. W., Tierney, S. C., Minami, T., & Wampold, B. E. (2003). Establishing specificity in
psychotherapy: A meta-analysis of structural equivalence of placebo controls. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71, 973–979.
Baucom, D. H., Shoham, V., Mueser, K. T., Daiuto, A.D., & Stickle, T. R. (1998). Empirically
supported couple and family interventions for marital distress and adult mental health
problems. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 53–88.
Baumeister, R. F. (2005). The cultural animal: Human nature, meaning, and social life.
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Beck, A. T., & Bhar, S. S. (2009). Effectiveness of long-term psychodynamic psychotherapy:
A meta-analysis: Comment. Journal of the American Medical Association, 301(9). doi:
10.1001/jama.2009.179
Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive therapy of depression.
New York: Guilford.
Beck, A. T., Ward, C., Mendelson, M., & Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring
depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 6, 561–571.
Beecher, H. K. (1955). The powerful placebo. Journal of the American Medical Association,
159(17), 1602–1606.
Bell, E. C., Marcus, D. K., & Goodlad, J. K. (2013). Are the parts as good as the whole? A
meta-analysis of component treatment studies. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 81(4), 722–736. doi: 10.1037/a0033004
Benedetti, F. (2009). Placebo effects: Understanding the mechanisms in health and disease.
New York: Oxford University Press.
Benedetti, F. (2011). The patient’s brain: The neuroscience behind the doctor-patient
relationship. New York: Oxford University Press.
Benish, S. G., Imel, Z. E., & Wampold, B. E. (2008). The relative efficacy of bona fide
psychotherapies for treating post-traumatic stress disorder: A meta-analysis of direct
comparisons. Clinical Psychology Review, 28, 746–758.
Benish, S. G., Quintana, S., & Wampold, B. E. (2011). Culturally adapted psychotherapy and
the legitimacy of myth: A direct-comparison meta-analysis. Journal of Counseling
Psychology, 58(3), 279–289. doi: 10.1037/a0023626
Benjamin, L. S. (1994). SASB: A bridge between personality theory and clinical psychology.
Psychological Inquiry, 5, 273–316.
Bergin, A. E. (1963). The effects of psychotherapy: Negative results revisited. Journal of
Counseling Psychology, 10, 244–250.
Bergin, A. E. (1971). The evaluation of therapeutic outcomes. In S. L.Garfield & A. E.Bergin
(Eds.), Handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change (pp. 217–270). New York: Wiley.
Bergin, A. E., & Lambert, M. J. (1978). The evaluation of therapeutic outcomes. In S.
L.Garfield & A. E.Bergin (Eds.), Handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change: An
empirical analysis (2nd ed., pp. 139–190). New York: Wiley.
Berglund, M., Thelander, S., Salaspuro, M., Franck, J., Andréasson, S., & Öjehagen, A.
(2003). Treatment of alcohol abuse: An evidence-based review. Alcoholism: Clinical and
Experimental Research, 27, 1645–1656.
Berman, J. S., Miller, C., & Massman, P. J. (1985). Cognitive therapy versus systematic
desensitization: Is one treatment superior? Psychological Bulletin, 97, 451–461.
Beutler, L. E. (1998). Identifying empirically supported treatments: What if we didn’t? Journal
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 113–120.
Beutler, L. E., & Baker, M. (1998). The movement toward empirical validation. In K. S.Dobson
& K. D.Craig (Eds.), Empirically supported therapies: Best practice in professional psychology
(pp. 43–65). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Beutler, L. E., & Castonguay, L. G. (Eds.). (2006). Principles of therapeutic change that work.
New York: Oxford.
Beutler, L. E., & Clarkin, J. (1990). Differential treatment selection: Toward targeted
therapeutic interventions. New York: Brunner/Mazel.
Beutler, L. E., Frank, M., Schieber, S. C., Calvert, L., & Gaines, J. (1984). Comparative
effects of group psychotherapies in a short-term inpatient setting: An experience with
deterioration effects . Psychiatry, 44, 67–76.
Beutler, L. E., & Harwood, T. M. (2000). Perscriptive psychotherapy: A practical guide to
systematic treatment selection. New York: Oxford University Press.
Beutler, L. E., Harwood, T. M., Alimohamed, S., & Malik, M. (2002). Functional impairment
and coping style. In J. C.Norcross (Ed.), Psychotherapy relationships that work: Therapist
contributions and responsiveness to patients (pp. 145–170). New York: Oxford University.
Beutler, L. E., Harwood, T. M., Kimpara, S., Verdirame, D., & Blau, K. (2011). Coping style.
Journal of Clinical Psychology, 67(2), 176–183. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20752
Beutler, L. E., Harwood, T. M., Michelson, A., Song, X., & Holman, J. (2011).
Resistance/reactance level. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 67(2), 133–142. doi:
10.1002/jclp.20753
Beutler, L. E., Malik, M., Alimohamed, S., Harwood, T. M., Talebi, H., Noble, S., & Wong, E.
(2004). Therapist variables. In M. J.Lambert (Ed.), Bergin and Garfield’s handbook of
psychotherapy and behavior change (5th ed., pp. 227–306). New York: Wiley.
Bhar, S. S., & Beck, A. T. (2009). Treatment integrity of studies that compare short-term
psychodynamic psychotherapy with cognitive-behavior therapy. Clinical Psychology: Science
and Practice, 16(3), 370–378. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2850.2009.01176.x
Bisson, J. I., Ehlers, A., Matthews, R., Pilling, S., Richards, D., & Turner, S. (2007).
Psychological treatments for chronic post-traumatic stress disorder: Systematic review and
meta-analysis. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 190(2), 97–104.
doi:10.1192/bjp.bp.106.021402
Bisson, J., & Andrew, M. (2009). Psychological Treatment of Post-traumatic Stress Disorder
(PTSD) (Review). The Cochrane Library, 2, www.thecochranelibrary.com
Blatt, S. J., Sanislow III, C. A., Zuroff, D. C., & Pilkonis, P. A. (1996). Characteristics of
effective therapists: further analyses of data from the National Institute of Mental Health
Treatment of Depression Collaborative Research Program. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
psychology, 64(6), 1276.
Bleiberg, K. L., & Markowitz, J. C. (2005). A pilot study of interpersonal psychotherapy for
posttraumatic stress disorder. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 162(1), 181–183. doi:
10.1176/appi.ajp.162.1.181
Boffey, P. M. (1982, June 28). Alcoholism study under new attack. New York Times, pp. 12.
Bohanske, R. T., & Franczak, M. (2010). Transforming public behavioral health care: A case
example of consumer-directed services, recovery, and the common factors. In B. L.Duncan,
S. D.Miller, B. E.Wampold & M. A.Hubble (Eds.), The heart and soul of change: Delivering
what works in therapy (2nd ed., pp. 299–322). Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.
Bohart, A. C., & Tallman, K. (1999). How Clients Make Therapy Work: The Process of Active
Self-Healing. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Bohart, A. C., & Wade, A. G. (2013). The client in psychotherapy. In M. J.Lambert (Ed.),
Bergin and Garfield’s handbook of pyschotherapy and behavior change (6th ed., pp.
219–257). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Bordin, E. S. (1979). The generalizability of the psychoanalytic concept of the working
alliance. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research & Practice, 16(3), 252–260. doi:
10.1037/h0085885
Borkovec, T. D. (1990). Control groups and comparison groups in psychotherapy outcome
research. National Institute on Drug Abuse Research Monograph, 104, 50–65.
Borkovec, T. D., & Castonguay, L. G. (1998). What is the scientific meaning of empirically
supported therapy? Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 136–142.
Borkovec, T. D., & Costello, E. (1993). Efficacy of applied relaxation and cognitive-behavioral
therapy in the treatment of generalized anxiety disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 61, 611–619.
Boswell, J. F., Gallagher, M. W., Sauer-Zavala, S. E., Bullis, J., Gorman, J. M., Shear, M. K.,
… Barlow, D. H. (2013). Patient characteristics and variability in adherence and competence
in cognitive-behavioral therapy for panic disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 81(3), 443–454. doi: 10.1037/a0031437
Boswell, J. F., Kraus, D. R., Miller, S. D., & Lambert, M. J. (in press). Implementing routine
outcome monitoring in clinical practice: Benefits, challenges, and solutions. Psychotherapy
Research.
Bowers, T. G., & Clum, G. A. (1988). Relative contributions of specific and nonspecific
treatment effects: Meta-analysis of placebo-controlled behavior therapy research.
Psychological Bulletin, 103, 315–323.
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss. Vol. 1: Attachment. New York: Basic Books.
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss, Vol. 2: Separation-anxiety and anger. New York:
Basic Books.
Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss, Vol. 3: Loss-sadness and depression. New York:
Basic Books.
Boyer, P., & Barrett, H. C. (2005). Domain specificity and intuitive ontologies. In D. M.Buss
(Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 96–118). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Bradley, R., Greene, J., Russ, E., Dutra, L., & Westen, D. (2005). A multidimensional meta-
analysis of psychotherapy for PTSD. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 214–227.
Braun, S.R., Gregor, B., Tran, U.S. (2013). Comparing bona fide psychotherapies of
depression in adults with two meta-analytical approaches. PLoS ONE, 8(6):e68135. doi:
10.1371/journal.pone.0068135
Brody, N. (1980). Placebos and the philosophy of medicine: Clinical, conceptual, and ethical
issues. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Budge, S. L., Baardseth, T. P., Wampold, B. E., & Flückiger, C. (2010). Researcher
allegiance and supportive therapy: Pernicious affects on results of randomized clinical trials.
European Journal of Counselling and Psychotherapy, 12, 23–39.
Budge, S. L., Moore, J. T., Del Re, A. C., Wampold, B. E., Baardseth, T. P., & Nienhuis, J. B.
(2013). The effectiveness of evidence-based treatments for personality disorders when
comparing treatment-as-usual and bona fide treatments. Clinical Psychology Review, 33,
1057–1066. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2013.08.003
Buranelli, V. (1975). The wizard from Vienna: Franz Anton Mesmer. New York: Coward,
McCann & Geoghegan.
Burns, D. D., & Spangler, D. L. (2001). Do changes in dysfunctional attitudes mediate
changes in depression and anxiety in cognitive behavioral therapy? Behavior Therapy, 32(2),
337–369. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7894(01)80008-3
Butler, A. C., Chapman, J. E., Forman, E. M., & Beck, A. T. (2006). The empirical status of
cognitive-behavioral therapy: A review of meta-analyses. Clinical Psychology Review, 26,
17–31. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2005.07.003
Butler, G., Fennell, M., Robson, P., & Gelder, M. (1991). Comparison of behavior therapy and
cognitive behavior therapy in the treatment of generalized anxiety disorder. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59(1) 167–175.
Cacioppo, J. T., Fowler, J. H., & Christakis, N. A. (2009). Alone in the crowd: The structure
and spread of loneliness in a large social network. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 97(6), 977–991. doi: 10.1037/a0016076
Cacioppo, S., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2012). Decoding the invisible forces of social connections.
Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 6. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2012.00051
Calhoun, K. S., Moras, K., Pilkonis, P. A., & Rehm, L. (1998). Empirically supported
treatments: Implications for training. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66,
151–161.
Campbell, D. T., & Kenny, D. A. (1999) A primer on regression artifacts. New York: Guilford.
Caplan, E. (1998). Mind games: American culture and the birth of psychotherapy. Berkeley:
University of California Press.
Carbajal, R., Chauvet, X., Couderc, S., & Oliver-Martin, M. (1999). Randomised trial of
analgesic effects of sucrose, glucose, and pacifiers in term neonates. British Medical Journal,
319, 1393–1397.
Carroll, K. M., Rounsaville, B. J., & Nich, C. (1994). Blind man’s bluff: Effectiveness and
significance of psychotherapy and pharmacoptherapy blinding procedures in a clinical trial.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 276–280.
Castonguay, L. G. (1993). “Common factors” and “nonspecific variables”: Clarification of the
two concepts and recommendations for research. Journal of Psychotherapy Integration, 3,
267–286.
Castonguay, L. G., & Beutler, L. E. (Eds.). (2006). Principles of therapeutic change that work.
New York: Oxford University Press.
Castonguay, L. G., Goldfried, M. R., Wiser, S., Raue, P. J., & Hayes, A. M. (1996). Predicting
the effect of cognitive therapy for depression: A study of unique and common factors. Journal
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 497–504.
Chambless, D. L., Baker, M. J., Baucom, D. H., Beutler, L. E., Calhoun, K. S., Daiuto, A.,
(1998). Update on empirically validated therapies, II. The Clinical Psychologist, 51, 3–16.
Chambless, D. L., & Crits-Christoph, P. (2006). What should be validated? The treatment
method. In J. C.Norcross, L. E.Beutler & R. F.Levant (Eds.), Evidence-based practices in
mental health: Debate and dialogue on the fundamental questions (pp. 191–200).
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Chambless, D. L., & Gillis, M. M. (1993). Cognitive therapy of anxiey disorders. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61, 248–260.
Chambless, D. L., & Hollon, S. D. (1998). Defining empirically supported therapies. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 7–18.
Chambless, D. L., Sanderson, W. C., Shoham, V., Johnson, S. B., Pope, K. S., Crits-
Christoph, P., (1996). An update on empirically validated therapies. The Clinical Psychologist,
49(2), 5–18.
Christakis, N. A., & Fowler, J. H. (2007). The spread of obesity in a large social network over
32 years. The New England Journal of Medicine, 357(4), 370–379. doi:
10.1056/NEJMsa066082
Clark, D. M. (2013). Psychodynamic therapy or cognitive therapy for social anxiety disorder.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 170(11), 1365.
Clark, D. M., Ehlers, A., Hackmann, A., McManus, F., Fennell, M., Grey, N., … & Wild, J.
(2006). Cognitive therapy versus exposure and applied relaxation in social phobia: a
randomized controlled trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74(3) 568–578.
Clark, D. M., Ehlers, A., McManus, F., Hackmann, A., Fennell, M., Campbell, H., … & Louis,
B. (2003). Cognitive therapy versus fluoxetine in generalized social phobia: a randomized
placebo-controlled trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71(6) 1058–1067.
Clark, D. M., Fairburn, C. G., & Wessely, S. (2008). Psychological treatment outcomes in
routine NHS services: A commentary on Stiles et al. (2007). Psychological Medicine, 38,
629–634. doi: 10.1017/S0033291707001869
Clark, D. M., Salkovskis, P. M., Hackmann, A., Middleton, H., Anastasiades, P., & Gelder, M.
(1994). A comparison of cognitive therapy, applied relaxation, and imipramine in the
treatment of panic disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 759–769.
Clarkin, J. F., & Levy, K. N. (2004). The infuence of client variables on psychotherapy. In M.
J.Lambert (Ed.), Bergin and Garfield’s handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change (5th
ed., pp. 194–226). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Clement, P. W. (1994). Quantitative evaluation of 26 years of private practice. Professional
Psychology: Research and Practice, 25(2), 173–176. doi: 10.1037/0735-7028.25.2.173
Clement, P. W. (1996). Evaluation in private practice. Clinical Psychology: Science and
Practice, 3(2), 146–159. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2850.1996.tb00064.x
Clum, G. A., Clum, G. A., & Surls, R. (1993). A meta-analysis of treatments for panic
disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61, 317–326.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale,
NJ: Erlbaum.
Cohen, A. S., Barlow, D. H., & Blanchard, E. B. (1985). Psychophysiology of relaxation-
associated panic attacks. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 94, 96–101.
Cohen, S., & Syme, S. L. (1985). Social support and health. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Compas, B. E., Haaga, D.A.F., Keefe, F. J., Leitenberg, H., & Williams, D. A. (1998).
Sampling of empirically supported psychological treatments from health psychology:
Smoking, chronic pain, cancer, and bulimia nervosa. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 66, 89–112.
Connell, J., Grant, S., & Mullin, T. (2006). Client initiated termination of therapy at NHS
primary care counselling services. Counselling & Psychotherapy Research, 6(1), 60–67. doi:
10.1080/14733140600581507
Constantino, M. J., Arnkoff, D. B., Glass, C. R., Ametrano, R. M., & Smith, J. Z. (2011).
Expectations. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 67(2), 184–192. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20754
Constantino, M. J., Glass, C. R., Arnkoff, D. B., Ametrano, R. M., & Smith, J. Z. (2011).
Expectations. In J. C.Norcross (Ed.), Psychotherapy relationships that work: Evidence-based
responsiveness (2nd ed.). (pp. 354–376). New York: Oxford University Press.
Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (1979). Quasi-experimentation: Design and analysis for field
settings. Chicago: Rand McNally.
Cooper, H., & Hedges, L. V. (Eds.). (1994). The handbook of research synthesis. New York:
Russell Sage Foundation.
Cooper, H., Hedges, L. V., & Valentine, J. C. (Eds.). (2009). The handbook of research
synthesis and meta-analysis (2nd ed.). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.
Craske, M. G., Meadows, E. A., & Barlow, D. H. (1994). Therapist guide for the mastery of
your anxiety and panic II and agoraphobia supplement. Albany, NY: Graywind Publications
Incorporated.
Cremer, S., & Sixt, M. (2009). Analogies in the evolution of individual and social immunity.
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364 (1513), 129–142.
Critelli, J. W., & Neumann, K. F. (1984). The placebo: Conceptual analysis of a construct in
transition. American Psychologist, 39, 32–39.
Crits-Christoph, P. (1997). Limitations of the dodo bird verdict and the role of clinical trials in
psychotherapy research: Comment on Wampold et al. (1997). Psychological Bulletin, 122,
216–220.
Crits-Christoph, P., Baranackie, K., Kurcias, J. S., Carroll, K., Luborsky, L., McLellan, T., …
Zitrin, C. (1991). Meta-analysis of therapist effects in psychotherapy outcome studies.
Psychotherapy Research, 1, 81–91.
Crits-Christoph, P., Gallop, R., Temes, C. M., Woody, G., Ball, S. A., Martino, S., & Carroll, K.
M. (2009). The alliance in motivational enhancement therapy and counseling as usual for
substance use problems. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 77(6), 1125–1135.
doi: 10.1037/a0017045
Crits-Christoph, P., Gibbons, M.B.C., Hamilton, J., Ring-Kurtz, S., & Gallop, R. (2011). The
dependability of alliance assessments: The alliance–outcome correlation is larger than you
might think. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 79(3), 267–278. doi:
10.1037/a0023668
Crits-Christoph, P., Gibbons, M. B., & Hearon, B. (2006). Does the alliance cause good
outcome? Recommendations for future research on the alliance. Psychotherapy: Theory,
Research, Practice, Training, 43(3), 280–285.
Crits-Christoph, P., Gibbons, M.B.C., Ring-Kurtz, S., Gallop, R., Stirman, S., Present, J., …
Goldstein, L. (2008). Changes in positive quality of life over the course of psychotherapy.
Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 45(4), 419–430. doi:
10.1037/a0014340
Crits-Christoph, P., & Mintz, J. (1991). Implications of therapist effects for the design and
analysis of comparative studies of psychotherapies. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 59(1) 20–26.
Cronbach, L. J., & Snow, R. E. (1977). Aptitudes and instructional methods: A handbook for
research on interactions. Oxford: Irvington.
Cuijpers, P. (1997). Bibliotherapy in unipolar depression: A meta-analysis. Journal of
Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 28(2), 139–147. doi: 10.1016/s0005-
7916(97)00005-0
Cuijpers, P., Driessen, E., Hollon, S. D., van Oppen, P., Barth, J., & Andersson, G. (2012).
The efficacy of non-directive supportive therapy for adult depression: A meta-analysis.
Clinical Psychology Review, 32, 280–291. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2012.01.003
Cuijpers, P., van Straten, A., Andersson, G., & van Oppen, P. (2008a). Psychotherapy for
depression in adults: A meta-analysis of comparative outcome studies. Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology, 76(6) 909–922. doi:10.1037/a0013075
Cuijpers, P., van Straten, A., Warmerdam, L., & Andersson, G. (2008b). Psychological
treatment of depression: A meta-analytic database of randomized studies. BMC Psychiatry,
8(1) 36. doi:10.1186/1471-244X-8-36
Curran, P. J., & Bauer, D. J. (2011). The disaggregation of within-person and between person
effects in longitudinal models of change. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 5833–5619. doi:
10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100356
Currier, J. M., Neimeyer, R. A., & Berman, J. S. (2008). The effectiveness of
psychotherapeutic interventions for bereaved persons: A comprehensive quantitative review.
Psychological Bulletin, 134, 648–661.
Cushman, P. (1992). Psychotherapy to 1992: A history situated interpretation. In D.
K.Freedheim (Ed.), History of psychotherapy: A century of change (pp. 21–64). Washington,
DC: American Psychological Association.
Dance, K. A., & Neufeld, R.W.J. (1988). Aptitude-treatment interaction research in the clinic
setting: A review of attempts to dispel the “patient uniformity” myth. Psychological Bulletin,
104, 192–213.
Danziger, K. (1990). Constructing the subject: Historical origins of psychological research.
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Davidson, P. R., & Parker, K.C.H. (2001). Eye movement desensitization and reprocessing
(EMDR): A meta-analysis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 69, 305–316.
Davies, D. L. (1962). Normal drinking in recovered addicts. Quarterly Journal of Studies on
Alcohol, 23, 94–104.
Davison, G. C. (1998). Being bolder with the Boulder Model: The challenge of education and
training in empirically supported treatments. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
66, 163–167.
De Bolle, M., Johnson, J. G., & De Fruyt, F. (2010). Patient and clinician perceptions of
therapeutic alliance as predictors of improvement in depression. Psychotherapy and
Psychosomatics, 79(6), 378–385. doi: 10.1159/000320895
de Waal, F.B.M. (2008). Putting the altruism back into altruism: The evolution of empathy.
Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 279–300. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093625
Del Re, A. C., Flückiger, C., Horvath, A. O., Symonds, D., & Wampold, B. E. (2012).
Therapist effects in the therapeutic alliance–outcome relationship: A restricted-maximum
likelihood meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 32(7), 642–649. doi:
10.1016/j.cpr.2012.07.002
Del Re, A. C., Spielmans, G. I., Flückiger, C., & Wampold, B. E. (2013). Efficacy of new
generation antidepressants: Differences seem illusory. PLoS ONE, 8(6): e63509. doi:
10.1371/journal.pone.0063509
DeRubeis, R. J., Brotman, M. A., & Gibbons, C. J. (2005). A Conceptual and Methodological
Analysis of the Nonspecifics Argument. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 12(2),
174–183.
DeRubeis, R. J., & Crits-Christoph, P. (1998). Empirically supported individual and group
psychological treatments for mental disorders. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
66, 37–52.
DeRubeis, R. J., & Feeley, M. (1990). Determinants of change in cognitive therapy for
depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 14, 469–482.
DeRubeis, R. J., Siegle, G. J., & Hollon, S. D. (2008). Cognitive therapy versus medication
for depressions: Treatment outcomes and neural mechanisms. Nature Reviews
Neuroscience, 9(10), 788–796. doi: 10.1038/nrn2345
Desrosières, A. (1998). The politics of large numbers: A history of statistical reasoning
(C.Naish, Trans.). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Devilly, G. J., & Foa, E. B. (2001). The investigation of exposure and cognitive therapy:
Comment on Tarrier et al. (1999). Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 69(1),
114–116. doi: 10.1037/0022-006x.69.1.114
Dinger, U., Strack, M., Leichsenring, F., Wilmers, F., & Schauenburg, H. (2008). Therapist
effects on outcome and alliance in inpatient psychotherapy. Journal of Clinical Psychology,
64(3), 344–354. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20443
Dishion, T. J., McCord, J., & Poulin, F. O. (1999). When interventions harm: Peer groups and
problem behavior. American Psychologist, 54, 755–764.
Dobson, K. S. (1989). A meta-analysis of the efficacy of cognitive therapy for depression.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, 414–419.
Dollard, J., & Miller, N. E. (1950). Personality and psychotherapy: An analysis in terms of
learning, thinking, and culture. New York: McGraw Hill.
Duncan, B. L., Miller, S. D., & Sparks, J. A. (2004). The heroic client: a revolutionary way to
improve effectiveness through client-directed, outcome-informed therapy (Rev.ed.). San
Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Duncan, B. L., Miller, S. D., Wampold, B. E., & Hubble, M. A. (2010). The heart and soul of
change: Delivering what works in therapy (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Dush, D. M., Hirt, M. L., & Schroeder, H. (1983). Self-statement modification with adults: A
meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 94, 408–422.
Ehlers, A., Bisson, J., Clark, D. M., Creamer, M., Pilling, S., Richards, D., … Yule, W. (2010).
Do all psychological treatments really work the same in post-traumatic stress disorder?
Clinical Psychology Review, 30(2), 269–276. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.12.001
Elkin, I. (1994). The NIMH Treatment of Depression Collaborative Research Program: Where
we began and where we are. In A. E.Bergin & S. L.Garfield (Eds.), Handbook of
psychotherapy and behavior change (4th ed., pp. 114–139). New York: Wiley.
Elkin, I., Gibbons, R. D., Shea, M. T., & Shaw, B. F. (1996). Science is not a trial (but it can
sometimes be a tribulation). Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 92–103.
Elkin, I., Parloff, M. B., Hadley, S. W., & Autry, J. H. (1985). NIMH treatment of depression
collaborative research program: Background and research plan. Archives of General
Psychiatry, 42(3) 305–316.
Elkin, I., Shea, T., Watkins, J. T., Imber, S. D., Sotsky, S. M., Collins, J. F., … Parloff, M. B.
(1989). National Institute of Mental Health Treatment of Depression Collaborative Research
Program. Archives of General Psychiatry, 46, 971–982.
Ellickson, P. L., Bell, R. M., & McGuigan, K. (1993). Preventing adolescent drug use: Long-
term results of a junior high program. American Journal of Public Health, 83, 856–861.
Elliott, R., Bohart, A. C., Watson, J. C., & Greenberg, L. S. (2011). Empathy. Psychotherapy,
48(1), 43–49. doi: 10.1037/a0022187
Elliott, R., Greenberg, L. S., Watson, J., Timulak, L., & Freire, E. (2013). Research on
humanistic-experiential psychotherapies. In M. J.Lambert (Ed.), Bergin and Garfield’s
handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change (6th ed., pp. 495–538). New York: Wiley.
Ellis, A. (1957). Outcome of emplying three techniques of psychotehrapy. Journal of Clinical
Psychology, 13, 344–350.
Ellison, J. A., & Greenberg, L. S. (2007). Emotion-focused experiential therapy. New York:
Springer Science.
Ellsworth, J. R., Lambert, M. J., & Johnson, J. (2006). A comparison of the Outcome
Questionnaire-45 and Outcome Questionnaire-30 in classification and prediction of treatment
outcome. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 13(6), 380–391.
Emmelkamp, P.M.G. (2004). Behavior therapy with adults. In M.Lambert (Ed.), Bergin and
Garfield’s handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change (5th ed., pp. 393–446). Oxford:
Wiley & Sons.
Emmelkamp, P.M.G. (2013). Behavior therapy with adults. In M. J.Lambert (Ed.), Bergin and
Garfield’s handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change (6th ed., pp. 343–392). New
York: Wiley.
Eysenck, H. J. (1952). The effects of psychotherapy: An evaluation. Journal of Consulting
Psychology, 16, 319–324.
Eysenck, H. J. (1961). The effects of psychotherapy. In H. J.Eysenck (Ed.), Handbook of
abnormal psychology (pp. 697–725). New York: Basic Books.
Eysenck, H. J. (1966). The effects of psychotherapy. New York: International Science Press.
Eysenck, H. J. (1978). An exercise in meta-silliness. American Psychologist, 33, 517.
Eysenck, H. J. (1984). Meta-analysis: An abuse of research integration. The Journal of
Special Education, 18(1) 41–59.
Falkenström, F., Granström, F., & Holmqvist, R. (2013). Therapeutic alliance predicts
symptomatic improvement session by session. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 60(3),
317–328. doi: 10.1037/a0032258
Falkenström, F., Granström, F., & Holmqvist, R. (2014). Working alliance predicts
psychotherapy outcome even while controlling for prior symptom improvement.
Psychotherapy Research, 24(2) 146–159. doi:10.1080/10503307.2013.847985
Falkenström, F., Markowitz, J. C., Jonker, H., Philips, B., & Holmqvist, R. (2013). Can
psychotherapists function as their own controls? Meta-analysis of the crossed therapist
design in comparative psychotherapy trials. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 74(5), 482–491.
doi: 10.4088/JCP.12r07848
Farber, B. A., & Doolin, E. M. (2011). Positive regard and affirmation. In J. C.Norcross (Ed.),
Psychotherapy relationships that work: Evidence-based responsiveness (2nd ed., pp.
168–186). New York: Oxford University Press.
Feeley, M., DeRubeis, R. J., & Gelfand, L. A. (1999). The temporal relation of adherence and
alliance to symptom change in cognitive therapy for depression. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 67, 578–582.
Fisher, R. A. (1935). The design of experiments. Edinburgh: Oliver and Boyd.
Fisher, S., & Greenberg, R. P. (1997). The curse of the placebo: Fanciful pursuit of a pure
biological therapy. In S.Fisher & R. P.Greenberg (Eds.), From placebo to panacea: Putting
psychiatric drugs to the test (pp. 3–56). New York: Wiley.
Fishman, D. B., & Franks, C. M. (1992). Evolution and differentiation within behavior therapy:
A theoretical and epistemological review. In D. K.Freedheim (Ed.), History of psychotherapy:
A century of change (pp. 159–196). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Flückiger, C., Del Re, A. C., Horvath, A. O., Symonds, D., Ackert, M., & Wampold, B. E.
(2013). Substance use disorders and racial/ethnic minorities matter: A meta-analytic
examination of the relation between alliance and outcome. Journal of Counseling
Psychology, 60 (4), 610–616. doi: 10.1037/a0033161
Flückiger, C., Del Re, A. C., & Wampold, B. E. (in press). The Sleeper Effect: Artifact or
Phenomenon—A brief comment on Are the Parts as Good as the Whole? A Meta-Analysis of
Component Treatment Studies (Bell, Marcus & Goodlad, 2013). Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology.
Flückiger, C., Del Re, A. C., Wampold, B. E., Symonds, D., & Horvath, A. O. (2012). How
central is the alliance in psychotherapy? A multilevel longitudinal meta-analysis. Journal of
Counseling Psychology, 59 (1), 10–17. doi: 10.1037/a0025749
Flückiger, C., Holtforth, M. G., Znoj, H. J., Caspar, F., & Wampold, B. E. (2013). Is the
relation between early post-session reports and treatment outcome an epiphenomenon of
intake distress and early response? A multi-predictor analysis in outpatient psychotherapy.
Psychotherapy Research, 23 (1), 1–13. doi: 10.1080/10503307.2012.693773
Foa, E. B., Gillihan, S. J., & Bryant, R. A. (2013). Challenges and successes in dissemination
of evidence-based treatments for posttraumatic stress: Lessons learned from prolonged
exposure therapy for PTSD. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 14 (2), 65–111. doi:
10.1177/1529100612468841
Foa, E. B., Hembree, E. A., & Rothbaum, B. O. (2007). Prolonged exposure therapy for
PTSD: Emotional processing of traumatic experiences: Therapist guide. New York: Oxford
University Press.
Foa, E. B., & Kozak, M. J. (1986). Emotional processing of fear: Exposure to corrective
information. Psychological Bulletin, 99 (1), 20–35. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.99.1.20
Foa, E. B., Rothbaum, B. O., Riggs, D. S., & Murdock, T. B. (1991). Treatment of post-
traumatic stress disorder in rape victims: A comparison between cognitive-behavioral
procedures and counseling. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59, 715–723.
Follette, W. C., & Houts, A. C. (1996). Models of scientific progress and the role of theory in
taxonomy development: A case study of the DSM. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 64, 1120–1132.
Fortner, B. V. (1999). The effectiveness of grief counseling and therapy: A quantitative
review. University of Memphis, Memphis, TN.
Fowler, J. H., & Christakis, N. A. (2009). Dynamic spread of happiness in a large social
network: Longitudinal analysis over 20 years in the Framingham Heart Study. British Medical
Journal, 338 (7685), 1–13. doi: 10.1136/bmj.b1
Fowler, J. H., & Christakis, N. A. (2010). Cooperative behavior cascades in human social
networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America,
107 (12), 5334–5338. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0913149107
Frank, J. D. (1961). Persuasion and healing: A comparative study of psychotherapy.
Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.
Frank, J. D. (1973). Persuasion and healing: A comparative study of psychotherapy (Rev.
Ed.ed.). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.
Frank, J. D. (1992). Historical developments in research centers: The Johns Hopkins
Psychotherapy Research Project. In D. K.Freedheim (Ed.), History of psychotherapy: A
century of change (pp. 392–396). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Frank, J. D., & Frank, J. B. (1991). Persuasion and healing: A comparative study of
psychotherapy (3rd ed.). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.
Free, M. L., & Oei, T. P. (1989). Biological and psychological processes in the treatment and
maintenance of depression. Clinical Psychology Review, 9, 653–688.
French, T. M. (1933). Interrelations between psychoanalysis and the experimental work of
Pavlov. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 12, 1165–1203.
Friedlander, M. L., Escudero, V., Heatherington, L., & Diamond, G. M. (2011). Alliance in
couple and family therapy. Psychotherapy, 48 (1), 25–33. doi: 10.1037/a0022060
Frost, N. D., Laska, K. M., & Wampold, B. E. (2014). The evidence for present-centered
therapy as a treatment for posttraumatic stress disorder: Present-centered therapy. Journal of
Traumatic Stress, 27 (1), 1–8. doi:10.1002/jts.21881
Gabbard, G. O., Beck, J. S., & Holmes, J. (2005). Oxford textbook of psychotherapy. New
York: Oxford University Press.
Gaffan, E. A., Tsaousis, I., Kemp-Wheeler, S. M. (1995). Researcher allegiance and meta-
analysis: the case of cognitive therapy for depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 63 (6), 966–980.
Gallo, D. A., & Finger, S. (2000). The power of a musical instrument: Franklin, the Mozarts,
Mesmer, and the glass harmonica. History of Psychology, 3, 326–343.
Gardner, R. (1998). The brain and communication are basic for human clinical sciences.
British Journal of Medical Psychology, 71, 493–508.
Garfield, S. L. (1992). Eclectic pyschotherapy: A common factors approach. In J. C.Norcross
& M. R.Goldfried (Eds.), Handbook of psychotherapy integration (pp. 169–201). New York:
Basic Books.
Garfield, S. L. (1994). Research on client variables in psychotherapy. In A. E.Bergin & S.
L.Garfield (Eds.), Handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change (4th ed., pp. 191–228).
New York: Wiley.
Garfield, S. L. (1995). Psychotherapy: An eclectic-integrative approach. New York: Wiley &
Sons.
Garfield, S. L. (1998). Some comments on empirically supported treatments. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 121–125.
Gaston, L., Marmar, C. R., Gallagher, D., & Thompson, L. W. (1991). Alliance prediction of
outcome beyond in-treatment symptomatic change as psychotherapy processes.
Psychotherapy Research, 1 (2), 104–112. doi: 10.1080/10503309112331335531
Gauld, A. (1992). A history of hypnotism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Gehan, E., & Lemak, N. A. (1994). Statistics in medical research: Developments in clinical
trials. New York: Plenum Medical Book.
Gelso, C. (2014). A tripartite model of the therapeutic relationship: Theory, research, and
practice. Psychotherapy Research, 24 (2), 117–131. doi:10.1080/10503307.2013.845920
Gelso, C. J. (2009). The real relationship in a postmodern world: Theoretical and empirical
explorations. Psychotherapy Research, 19 (3), 253–264. doi: 10.1080/10503300802389242
Gelso, C. J., & Carter, J. A. (1994). Components of the psychotherapy relationship: Their
interaction and unfolding during treatment. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 41 (3),
296–306. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.41.3.296
Gelso, C. J., & Hayes, J. A. (2007). Countertransference and the therapist’s inner experience:
Perils and possibilities. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
Gilbert, P. (2010). Compassion focused therapy: Distinctive features. New York:
Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group.
Gilboa-Schechtman, E., Foa, E. B., Shafran, N., Aderka, I. M., Powers, M. B., Rachamim, L.,
… & Apter, A. (2010). Prolonged exposure versus dynamic therapy for adolescent PTSD: a
pilot randomized controlled trial. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent
Psychiatry, 49 (10), 1034–1042.
Glass, G. V. (1976). Primary, secondary, and meta-analysis of research. Educational
Researcher, 5, 3–8.
Gleick, J. (2003). Isaac Newton. New York: Pantheon Books.
Gleser, L. J., & Olkin, I. (2009). Stochastically dependent effect sizes. In H.Cooper, L.
V.Hedges & J. C.Valentine (Eds.), The handbook of research synthesis and meta-analysis
(2nd ed., pp. 357–376). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.
Gloaguen, V., Cottraux, J., Cucherat, M., Blackburn, I. (1998). A meta-analysis of the effects
of cognitive therapy in depressed patients. Journal of Affective Disorders, 49, 59–72.
Goldfried, M. R. (1980). Toward the delineation of therapeutic change principles. American
Psychologist, 35, 991–999.
Goldfried, M. R., & Wolfe, B. E. (1996). Psychotherapy practice and research: Repairing a
strained alliance. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 51, 1007–1016.
Goldstein, E., & Farmer, K. (Eds.) (1994). Confabulations: Creating false memories,
destroying families. Boca Raton, FL: SIRS Books.
Gould, S. J. (1989). The chain of reason vs. the chain of thumbs. Natural History, 7, 12–21.
Gould, S. J. (1991). Bully for Brontosaurus. New York: Norton.
Greenberg, L. S. (2007). A guide to conducting a task analysis of psychotherapeutic change.
Psychotherapy Research, 17, 15–30.
Greenberg, L. S. (2010). Emotion-focused therapy. Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.
Grencavage, L. M., & Norcross, J. C. (1990). Where are the commonalities among the
therapeutic common factors? Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 21, 372–378.
Greenberg, L. S. and Watson, J. C. (2005). Emotion-Focused Therapy for Depression.
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association Press.
Greenberg, L. S., & Webster, M. C. (1982). Resolving decisional conflict by Gestalt two-chair
dialogue: Relating process to outcome. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 29, 468–477.
Greenberg, R. P., Constantino, M. J., & Bruce, N. (2006). Are patient expectations still
relevant for psychotherapy process and outcome? Clinical Psychology Review, 26, 657–678.
Grissom, R. J. (1996). The magical number .7 + - .2: Meta-meta-analysis of the probability of
superior outcome in comparisons involving therapy, placebo, and control. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 973–982.
Grünbaum, A. (1981). The placebo concept. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 19, 157–167.
Guess, H. A., Kleinman, A., Kusek, J. W., & Engel, L. W. (2002). The science of placebo:
Toward an interdisciplinary research agenda. London: BMJ Books.
Harrington, A. (1997). The placebo effect: An interdiscipinary exploration. Cambridge, MA:
Harvard University Press.
Hatcher, R. L., & Barends, A. W. (1996). Patients’ view of the alliance in psychotherapy:
Exploratory factor analysis of three alliance measures. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 64 (6), 1326–1336. doi: 10.1037/0022-006x.64.6.1326
Hatcher, R. L., & Barends, A. W. (2006). How a Return to Theory Could Help Alliance
Research. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 43 (3), 292–299.
Hays, W. L. (1988). Statistics. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
Hedges, L. V. (1981). Distribution theory for Glass’s estimator of effect size and related
estimators. Journal of Educational Statistics, 6 (2), 107–128. doi: 10.2307/1164588
Hedges, L. V., & Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical methods for meta-analysis. San Diego: Academic
Press.
Heide, F. J., & Borkovec, T. D. (1984). Relaxation-induced anxiety: Mechanisms and
theoretical implications. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 22, 1–12.
Henry, W. P., Schacht, T. E., Strupp, H. H., Butler, S. F., & Binder, J. (1993). Effects of
training in time-limited dynamic psychotherapy: Mediators of therapists’ responses to training.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61, 441–447.
Henry, W. P., Strupp, H. H., Butler, S. F., Schacht, T. E., & Binder, J. (1993). Effects of
training in time-limited psychotherapy: Changes in therapist behavior. Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology, 61, 434–440.
Hentschel, E., Brandstätter, G., Dragosics, B., Hirschl, A. M., Nemec, H., Schütze, K., …
Wurzer, H. (1993). Effect of ranitidine and amoxicillin plus metronidazole on the eradication of
Helicobacter pylori and the recurrence of duodenal ulcer. The New England Journal of
Medicine, 328 (5), 308–312.
Heppner, P. P., & Claiborn, C. D. (1989). Social influence research in counseling: A review
and critique. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 36, 365–387.
Heppner, P. P., Kivlighan, D. M., & Wampold, B. E. (2008). Research design in counseling
(3rd ed.). Belmont, CA: Thomson Brooks/Cole.
Herbert, J. D., Lilienfeld, S. O., Lohr, J. M., Montgomery, R. W., O’Donohue, W. T., Rosen, G.
M., & Tolin, D. F. (2000). Science and pseudoscience in the development of eye movement
desensitization and reprocessing: Implications for clinical psychology. Clinical Psychology
Review, 20 (8), 945–971.
Hill, C. E. (1986). An overview of the Hill counselor and client verbal response modes
category systems. In L. S.Greenberg & W. M.Pinsof (Eds.), The psychotherapeutic process:
A research handbook (pp. 131–159). New York: Guilford.
Hill, C. E., O’Grady, K. E., & Elkin, I. (1992). Applying the Collaborative Study Psychotherapy
Rating Scale to rate therapist adherence in cognitive-behavior therapy, interpersonal therapy,
and clinical management. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 60, 73–79.
Hoffart, A., Borge, F.M., Sexton, H., Clark, D. M., & Wampold, B. E. (2012). Psychotherapy
for social phobia: How do alliance and cognitive process interact to produce outcome?
Psychotherapy Research, 22 (1), 82–94. doi: 10.1080/10503307.2011.626806
Hoffart, A., Øktedalen, T., Langkaas, T. F., & Wampold, B. E.. (2013). Alliance and outcome
in varying imagery procedures for PTSD: A study of within-person processes. Journal of
Counseling Psychology, 60 (4), 471–482. doi: 10.1037/a0033604
Hofmann, S. G. (2008). Common misconceptions about cognitive mediation of treatment
change: A commentary to Longmore and Worrell (2007). Clinical Psychology Review, 28 (1),
67–70. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2007.03.003
Hofmann, S. G., & Lohr, J. M. (2010). To kill a dodo bird. The Behavior Therapist, 33 (1),
14–15.
Holland, P. W. (1986). Statistics and causal inference. Journal of the American Statistical
Association, 81 (396), 945–960. doi: 10.2307/2289064
Holland, P. W. (1993). Which comes first, cause or effect? In G.Keren, C.Lewis, G.Keren &
C.Lewis (Eds.), A handbook for data analysis in the behavioral sciences: Methodological
issues. (pp. 273–282). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Hollon, S. D., & Beck, A. T. (2013). Cognitive and cognitive-behavioral therapies. In M.
J.Lambert (Ed.), Bergin and Garfield’s handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change (6th
ed., pp. 393–442). New York: Wiley.
Hollon, S. D., DeRubeis, R. J., & Evans, M. D. (1987). Causal mediation of change in
treatment for depression: Discriminating between nonspecificity and noncausality.
Psychological Bulletin, 102, 139–149.
Holt-Lunstad, J., Smith, T. B., & Layton, J. B. (2010). Social relationships and mortality risk: a
meta-analytic review. Plos Medicine, 7 (7), e1000316.
Honyashiki, M., Furukawa, T. A., Noma, H., Tanaka, S., Chen, P., Ono, M., … Caldwell, D.
M. (2014). Specificity of CBT for depression: A contribution from multiple treatments meta-
analyses. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 38 249–260, doi: 10.1007/s10608-014-9599-7
Horvath, A. O. (2006). The alliance in context: Accomplishments, challenges, and future
directions. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 43 (3), 258–263. doi:
10.1037/0033-3204.43.3.258
Horvath, A. O., & Bedi, R. P. (2002). The alliance. In J. C.Norcross (Ed.), Psychotherapy
relationships that work: Therapist contributions and responsiveness to patients (pp. 37–70).
New York: Oxford University.
Horvath, A. O., Del Re, A. C., Flückiger, C., Symonds, D. (2011a). Alliance in individual
psychotherapy. In J. C.Norcross (Ed.), Psychotherapy relationships that work: Evidence-
based responsiveness (2nd ed., pp. 25–69). New York: Oxford.
Horvath, A. O., Del Re, A. C., Flückiger, C., & Symonds, D. (2011b). Alliance in individual
psychotherapy. Psychotherapy, 48 (1), 9–16. doi: 10.1037/a0022186
Horvath, A. O., & Greenberg, L. S. (1989). Development and validation of the Working
Alliance Inventory. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 36 (2), 223–233. doi: 10.1037/0022-
0167.36.2.223
Horvath, A. O., & Symonds, B. D. (1991). Relation between working alliance and outcome in
psychotherapy: A meta-analysis. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 38, 139–149.
Horvath, P. (1988). Placebos and common factors in two decades of psychotherapy
research. Psychological Bulletin, 104, 214–225.
Howard, K. I., Krause, M. S., & Orlinsky, D. E. (1986). The attrition dilemma: Toward a new
strategy for psychotherapy research. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54,
106–110.
Howard, K. I., Krause, M. S., Saunders, S. M., & Kopta, S. M. (1997). Trials and tribulations
in the meta-analysis of treatment differences: Comment on Wampold (1997). Psychological
Bulletin, 122, 221–225.
Hoyt, W. T., & Del Re, A. C. (2013). Comparison of methods for aggregating dependent
effect sizes in meta-analysis. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Hoyt, W. T., & Larson, D. G. (2008). A realistic approach to drawing conclusions from the
scientific literature: Response to Bonanno and Lilienfeld. Professional Psychology Research
and Practice, 39 (3), 378–379.
Hróbjartsson, A., & Gøtzsche, P. C. (2001). Is the placebo powerless? An analysis of clinical
trials comparing placebo with no treatment. The New England Journal of Medicine, 344 (21),
1594–1602.
Hróbjartsson, A., & Gøtzsche, P. C. (2004). Is the placebo powerless? Update of a
systematic review with 52 new randomized trials comparing placebo with no treatment.
Journal of Internal Medicine, 256 (2), 91–100. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01355.x
Hróbjartsson, A., & Gøtzsche, P. C. (2006). Unsubstantiated claims of large effects of
placebo on pain: Serious errors in meta-analysis of placebo analgesia mechanism stuidies.
Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 59, 336–338.
Hróbjartsson, A., & Gøtzsche, P. C. (2007a). Powerful spin in the conclusion of Wampold et
al.’s re analysis of placebo versus no-treatment trials despite similar results as in original
review. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 63 (4), 373–377. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20357
Hróbjartsson, A., & Gøtzsche, P. C. (2007b). Wampold et al.’s reiterate spin in the conclusion
of a re-analysis of placebo versus no-treatment trials despite similar results as in original
review. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 63 (4), 405–408. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20356
Hubble, M. A., Duncan, B. L., & Miller, S. D. (Eds.). (1999). The heart & soul of change: What
works in therapy. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Huey, S. J., Jr, Tilley, J. L., Jones, E. O., & Smith, C. (in press). The contribution of cultural
competence to evdince-based care for ethnically diverse populations. Annual Review of
Clinical Psychology.
Hunsley, J., & Westmacott, R. (2007). Interpreting the magnitude of the placebo effect:
Mountain or molehill? Journal of Clinical Psychology, 63 (4), 391–399. doi:
10.1002/jclp.20352
Hunt, M. (1997). How science takes stock: The story of meta-analysis. New York: Russell
Sage Foundation.
Hunt, M., & Corman, R. (November 11, 1962). Analysis of Psychoanalysis. New York Times,
p. 248.
Ilardi, S. S., & Craighead, W. E. (1994). The role of nonspecific factors in cognitive-behavior
therapy for depression. Clinical Psychology, 1, 138–156.
Imber, S. D., Pilkonis, P. A., Sotsky, S. M., Elkin, I., Watkins, J. T., Collins, J. F., … Glass, D.
R. (1990). Mode-specific effects among three treatments for depression. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 58, 352–359.
Imel, Z. E., Baer, J. S., Martino, S., Ball, S. A., & Carroll, K. M. (2011). Mutual influence in
therapist competence and adherence to motivational enhancement therapy. Drug and
Alcohol Dependence, 115 (3), 229–236. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2010.11.010
Imel, Z. E., Barco, J. S., Brown, H., Baucom, B. R., Baer, J. S., Kircher, J., & Atkins, D. C.
(2014). Synchrony in vocally encoded arousal as an indicator of therapist empathy in
motivational interviewing. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 61 (1), 146–153.
Imel, Z.E., Sheng, E., Baldwin, S.A., & Atkins, D.C. (in press). Removing very low-performing
therapists: A simulation of performance-based retention in psychotherapy. Psychotherapy.
Imel, Z. E., Steyvers, M., Atkins, D.C. (in press). Computational Psychotherapy Research:
Scaling up the evaluation of patient provider interactions. Psychotherapy.
Imel, Z. E., Wampold, B. E., Miller, S. D., & Fleming, R. R. (2008). Distinctions without a
difference: Direct comparisons of psychotherapies for alcohol use disorders. Psychology of
Addictive Behaviors, 22 (4), 533–543. doi: 10.1037/a0013171
Jacobson, N. S. (1991). To be or not to be behavioral when working with couples: What does
it mean? Journal of Family Psychology, 4, 436–445.
Jacobson, N. S., Dobson, K. S., Truax, P. A., Addis, M. E., Koerner, K., Gollan, J. K., …
Price, S. E. (1996). A component analysis of cognitive-behavioral treatment for depression.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 295–304.
Jacobson, N. S., & Hollon, S. D. (1996a). Cognitive-behavior therapy versus
pharmacotherapy: Now that the jury’s returned its verdict, it’s time to present the rest of the
evidence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 74–80.
Jacobson, N. S., & Hollon, S. D. (1996b). Prospects for future comparisons between drugs
and psychotherapy: Lessons from the CBT-versus-pharmacotherapy exchange. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 104–108.
Johansson, P., & Høglend, P. (2007). Identifying mechanisms of change in psychotherapy:
Mediators of treatment outcome . Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 14 (1), 1–9. doi:
10.1002/cpp.514
Kaptchuk, T. J., Kelley, J. M., Conboy, L. A., Davis, R. B., Kerr, C. E., Jacobson, E. E., …
Lembo, A. J. (2008). Components of placebo effect: Randomised controlled trial in patients
with irritable bowel syndrome. BMJ: British Medical Journal, 336 (7651), 999–1003. doi:
10.1136/bmj.39524.439618.25
Karlin, B. E., & Cross, G. (2014). From the laboratory to the therapy room: National
dissemination and implementation of evidence-based psychotherapies in the U.S.
Department of Veterans Affairs Health Care System. American Psychologist, 69 (1), 19–33.
doi: 10.1037/a0033888
Kazdin, A. E. (1994) Methodology, design, and evaluation in pyschotherapy research. In
A.E.Bergin & S.L.Garfield (Eds.), Handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change (4th ed.,
pp. 19–71). New York: Wiley.
Kazdin, A. E. (2000). Psychotherapy for children and adolescents: Directions for research
and practice. New York: Oxford University Press.
Kazdin, A. E.. (2002 ). Research design in clinical psychology (4th ed.). Needham Heights,
MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Kazdin, A. E. (2007). Mediators and mechanisms of change in psychotherapy research.
Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 3, 1–27. doi:
10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091432
Kazdin, A. E. (2009). Understanding how and why psychotherapy leads to change.
Psychotherapy Research, 19 (4–5), 418–428. doi: 10.1080/10503300802448899
Kazdin, A. E. & Bass, D. (1989). Power to detect differences between alternative treatments
in comparative psychotherapy outcome research. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 57, 138–147.
Kazdin, A. E., Esveldt-Dawson, K., French, N. H., & Unis, A. S. (1987). Effects of parent
management training and problem-solving skills training combined in the treatment of
antisocial child behavior. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry,
26 (3), 416–424. doi: 10.1097/00004583-198705000-00024
Kazdin, A. E., & Weisz, J. R. (1998). Identifying and developing empirically supported child
and adolescent treatments. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 19–36.
Kelley, J. M., Lembo, A. J., Ablon, J. S., Villanueva, J. J., Conboy, L. A., Levy, R., …
Kaptchuk, T. J. (2009). Patient and practitioner influences on the placebo effect in irritable
bowel syndrome. Psychosomatic Medicine, 71 (7), 789–797. doi:
10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181acee12
Kendall, P. C. (1998). Empirically supported psychological therapies. Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology, 66, 3–6.
Kenny, D. A., & Judd, C. M. (1986). Consequences of violating the independence assumption
in analysis of variance. Psychological Bulletin, 99 (3), 422–431.
Kessler, R. C., Demler, O., Frank, R. G., Olfson, M., Pincus, H. A., Walters, E. E., …
Zaslavsky, A. M. (2005). Prevalence and Treatment of Mental Disorders, 1990 to 2003. New
England Journal of Medicine, 352, 2515–2523.
Kiesler, D. J. (1966). Some myths of psychotherapy research and the search for a paradigm.
Psychological Bulletin, 65 (2), 110–136. doi: 10.1037/h0022911
Kiesler, D. J. (1994). Standardization of intervention: The tie that binds psychotherapy
research and practice. In P. F.Talley, H. H.Strupp & S. F.Butler (Eds.), Psychotherapy
research and practice: Bridging the gap (pp. 143–153). New York: Basic Books.
Kiesler, D. J. (1996). Contemporary interpersonal theory and research: Personality,
psychopathology, and psychotherapy. Oxford: John Wiley and Sons.
Kim, D. M., Wampold, B. E., & Bolt, D. M. (2006). Therapist effects in psychotherapy: A
random effects modeling of the NIMH TDCRP data. Psychotherapy Research, 16, 161–172.
Kirk, R. E. (1995). Experimental design: Procedures for the behavioral sciences (3rd ed.).
Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole
Kirsch, I. (1985). Response expectancy as a determinant of experience and behavior.
American Psychologist, 40, 1189–1202.
Kirsch, I. (1999). How expectancies shape experience. Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Kirsch, I. (2000). Are drug and placebo effects in depression additive? Biological Psychiatry,
47 (8), 733–735. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(00)00832-5
Kirsch, I. (2002). Yes, there is a placebo effect, but is there a powerful antidepressant drug
effect? Prevention & Treatment, 5 (1), 22.
Kirsch, I. (2005). Placebo Psychotherapy: Synonym or Oxymoron? Journal of Clinical
Psychology, 61 (7), 791–803.
Kirsch, I. (2009). Antidepressants and the placebo response. Epidemiology and Psychiatric
Sciences, 18 (4), 318–322.
Kirsch, I. (2010). The emperor’s new drugs: Exploding the antidepressant myth. New York:
Basic Books.
Kirsch, I., Deacon, B. J., Huedo-Medina, T. B., Scoboria, A., Moore, T. J., & Johnson, B. T.
(2008). Initial severity and antidepressant benefits: A meta-analysis of data submitted to the
food and drug administration. Plos Medicine, 5 (2), 260–268. doi:
10.1371/journal.pmed.0050045
Kirsch, I., & Low, C. B. (2013). Suggestion in the treatment of depression. American Journal
of Clinical Hypnosis, 55 (3), 221–229. doi: 10.1080/00029157.2012.738613
Kirsch, I., Moore, T. J., Scoboria, A., & Nicholls, S. S. (2002). The emperor’s new drugs: An
analysis of antidepressant medication data submitted to the U.S. Food and Drug
Administration. Prevention & Treatment, 5, article 23.
Kirsch, I., & Sapirstein, G. (1998). Listening to Prozac but hearing placebo: A meta-analysis
of antidepressant medication. Prevention & Treatment, 1 (2), 2a. doi:10.1037/1522-
3736.1.1.12a
Kirsch, I., Scoboria, A., & Moore, T. J. (2002). Antidepressants and placebos: Secrets,
revelations, and unanswered questions. Prevention & Treatment, 5 (1), 33. doi:10.1037/1522-
3736.5.1.533r
Kivlighan, D. M., Jr., & Shaughnessy, P. (2000). Patterns of working alliance development: A
typology of client’s working alliance ratings. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 47 (3),
362–371. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.47.3.362
Klein, D. F. (1996). Preventing hung juries about therapy studies. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychoogy, 64, 81–87.
Klein, D. N., Schwartz, J. E., Santiago, N. J., Vivian, D., Vocisano, C., Castonguay, L. G., …
Keller, M. B. (2003). Therapeutic alliance in depression treatment: Controlling for prior
change and patient characteristics. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71 (6),
997–1006.
Klerman, G. L., Weissman, M. M., Rounsaville, B. J.., & Chevron, E.S. (1984). Interpersonal
psychotherapy of depression. New York: Basic Books.
Kohlenberg, R. J. & Tsai, M. (2007). Functional Analytic Psychotherapy: A guide for creating
intense and curative therapeutic relationships (2nd ed.). New York: Springer.
Kolden, G. G., Klein, M. H., Wang, C.-C., & Austin, S. B. (2011). Congruence/genuineness. In
J. C.Norcross (Ed.), Psychotherapy relationships that work: Evidence-based responsiveness
(2nd ed., pp. 187–202). New York: Oxford University Press.
Kong, J., Kaptchuk, T. J., Polich, G., Kirsch, I., Vangel, M., Zyloney, C., … Gollub, R. (2009).
Expectancy and treatment interactions: A dissociation between acupuncture analgesia and
expectancy evoked placebo analgesia. NeuroImage, 45 (3), 940–949. doi:
10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.12.025
Konrad, M., Vyleta, M. L., Theis, F. J., Stock, M., Tragust, S., Klatt, M., … Cremer, S. (2012).
Social transfer of pathogenic fungus promotes active immunisation in ant colonies. PLoS
Biology, 10 (4). doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1001300
Kraemer, H. C., & Kupfer, D. J. (2006). Size of treatment effects and their importance to
clinical research and practice. Biological Psychiatry, 2006, 990–996.
Kuhn, T. S. (1962). The structures of scientific revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago.
Kuhn, T. S. (1970). Logic of discovery or psychology of research. In I.Lakatos & A.Musgrave
(Eds.), Criticism and the growth of knowledge (pp. 1–23). Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
Lakatos, I. (1970). Falsification and the methodology of scientific research programmes. In
I.Lakatos & A.Musgrave (Eds.), Criticism and the growth of knowledge (pp. 91–196).
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Lakatos, I. (1976). Proofs and refutations: The logic of mathematical discovery. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Lakatos, I., & Musgrave, A. (Eds.). (1970). Criticism and the growth of knowledge.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Lambert, M. J. (1992). Psychotherapy outcome research: Implications for integrative and
eclectic therapists. In J. C.Norcross & M. R.Goldfried (Eds.), Handbook of psychotherapy
integration (pp. 94–129). New York: Basic Books.
Lambert, M. J. (2010). Prevention of treatment failure: The use of measuring, monitoring, and
feedback in clinical practice. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Lambert, M. J. (Ed.). (2013). Bergin and Garfield’s handbook of psychotherapy and behavior
change. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Lambert, M. J., & Bergin, A. E. (1994). The effectiveness of psychotherapy. In A. E.Bergin &
S. L.Garfield (Eds.), Handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change (4th ed., pp.
143–189). New York: Wiley.
Lambert, M. J., Bergin, A. E., & Collins, J. L. (1997). Therapist-induced deterioration in
psychotherapy. In A. S.Gurman & A.M.Razin (Eds.), The therapist’ s contributions to effective
treatment: An empirical assessment. New York: Pergamon.
Lambert, M. J., Gregersen, A. T., & Burlingame, G. M. (2004). The Outcome Questionnaire-
45. In M. E.Murish (Ed.), Use of psychological testing for treatment planning and outcome
assessment (3rd ed., Vol. 3, pp. 191–234). Mahway, NJ: Erlbaum.
Lambert, M. J., & Ogles, B. M. (2004). The efficacy and effectiveness of psychotherapy. In M.
J.Lambert (Ed.), Bergin and Garfield’s handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change (5th
ed., pp. 139–193). New York: Wiley.
Lambert, M. J., & Shimokawa, K. (2011). Collecting client feedback. In J. C.Norcross (Ed.),
Psychotherapy relationships that work: Evidence-based responsiveness (2nd ed., pp.
203–223). New York: Oxford University Press.
Landman, J. T., & Dawes, R. M. (1982). Psychotherapy outcome: Smith and Glass’
conclusions stand up under scrutiny. American Psychologist, 37 (5), 504–516. doi:
10.1037/0003-066X.37.5.504
Langman, P. F. (1997). White culture, Jewish culture, and the origins of psychotherapy.
Psychotherapy, 34, 207–218.
Larson, D. G., & Hoyt, W. T. (2007). What has become of grief counseling? An evaluation of
the empirical foundations of the new pessimism. Professional Psychology: Research and
Practice, 38, 347–355.
Larvor, B. (1998). Lakatos: An introduction. London: Routledge.
Laska, K. M., Gurman, A. S., & Wampold, B. E. (2014). Expanding the lens of evidence-
based practice in psychotherapy: A common factors perspective. Psychotherapy, 51,
467–481.
Laska, K. M., Smith, T. L., Wislocki, A. P., Minami, T., & Wampold, B. E. (2013). Uniformity of
evidence-based treatments in practice? Therapist effects in the delivery of cognitive
processing therapy for PTSD. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 60 (1), 31–41. doi:
10.1037/a0031294
Latour, B. (1999). Pandora’s hope: Essays on the reality of science studies. Cambridge, MA:
Harvard University Press.
Lau, A. S. (2006). Making the case for selective and directed cultural adaptations of
evidence-based treatments: Examples from parent training. Clinical Psychology: Science and
Practice, 13, 295–310.
Lazarus, A. A. (1981). The practice of multimodal therapy. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Leary, T. (1955). Psychiatry. Journal for the Study of Interpersonal Processes, 18, 147–161.
Leichsenring, F., Rabung, S., & Leibing, E. (2004). The efficacy of short-term psycho-
dynamic psychotherapy in specific psychiatric disorders: a meta-analysis. Archives of
General Psychiatry, 61,1208–1216.
Leichsenring, F., Salzer, S., Beutel, M. E., Herpertz, S., Hiller, W., Hoyer, J., … Leibing, E.
(2013). Psychodynamic therapy and cognitive-behavioral therapy in social anxiety disorder: A
multicenter randomized controlled trial. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 170 (7),
759–767.
Leichsenring, F., Salzer, S., & Leibing, E. (2013). Response to Clark. American Journal of
Psychiatry, 170 (11), 1365–1366.
Leykin, Y., & DeRubeis, R. J. (2009). Allegiance in psychotherapy outcome research:
Separating association from bias. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 16, 54–65.
Liberman, B. L. (1978). The role of mastery in psychotherapy: Maintenance of improvement
and prescriptive change. In J. D.Frank, R.Hoehn-Saric, S. D.Imber, B. L.Liberman & A.
R.Stone (Eds.), Effective ingredients of successful psychotherapy (pp. 35–72). Baltimore:
Johns Hopkins University Press.
Lieberman, M. D. (2013). Social: Why our brains are wired to connect. New York: Crown
Publishing Group.
Lilienfeld, S. O. (2007). Psychological treatments that cause harm . Perspectives on
Psychological Science, 2, 53–70.
Lilienfeld, S. O., Ritschel, L. A., Lynn, S. J., Cautin, R. L., & Latzman, R. D. (2013). Why
many clinical psychologists are resistant to evidence-based practice: Root causes and
constructive remedies. Clinical Psychology Review, 33 (7), 883–900. doi:
10.1016/j.cpr.2012.09.008
Lillard, A. (1998). Ethnopsychologies: Cultural variations in theories of mind. Psychological
Bulletin, 123 (1), 3–32. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.123.1.3
Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (1993). The efficacy of psychological, educational, and
behavioral treatment: confirmation from meta-analysis. American Psychologist, 48 (12),
1181–1209.
Loftus, E. F., & Davis, D. (2006). Recovered memories. Annual Review of Clinical
Psychology, 2, 469–498.
Longmore, R. J., & Worrell, M. (2007). Do we need to challenge thoughts in cognitive
behavior therapy? Clinical Psychology Review, 27 (2), 173–187. doi:
10.1016/j.cpr.2006.08.001
Luborsky, L. (1954). A note on Eysenck’s article “The effects of psychotherapy: an
evaluation.” British Journal of Psychology, 45, 129–131.
Luborsky, L., Crits-Christoph, P., McLellan, A. T., Woody, G., Piper, W., Liberman, B., … &
Pilkonis, P. (1986). Do therapists vary much in their success? Findings from four outcome
studies. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 56 (4), 501–512.
Luborsky, L., & DeRubeis, R. J. (1984). The use of psychotherapy treatment manuals: A
small revolution in psychotherapy research style. Clinical Psychology Review, 4, 5–14.
Luborsky, L., Diguer, L., Seligman, D. A., Rosenthal, R., Krause, E. D., Johnson, S., …
Schweizer, E. (1999). The researcher’s own therapy allegiances: A “wild card” in
comparisons of treatment efficacy. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 6 (1), 95–106.
Luborsky, L., McLellan, A. T., Diguer, L., Woody, G., & Seligman, D. A. (1997). The
Psychotherapist Matters: Comparison of Outcomes Across Twenty-Two Therapists and
Seven Patient Samples. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 4 (1), 53–65.
Luborsky, L., Singer, B., & Luborsky, L. (1975). Comparative studies of psychotherapies: Is it
true that “everyone has won and all must have prizes?” Archives of General Psychiatry, 32
(8), 995–1008.
Luo, Y., Hawkley, L. C., Waite, L. J., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2012). Loneliness, health, and
mortality in old age: A national longitudinal study. Social Science & Medicine, 74 (6),
907–914. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.11.028
MacCoon, D. G., Imel, Z. E., Rosenkranz, M. A., Sheftel, J. G., Weng, H. Y., Sullivan, J. C.,
… Lutz, A. (2012). The validation of an active control intervention for Mindfulness Based
Stress Reduction (MBSR). Behaviour Research and Therapy, 50 (1), 3–12. doi:
10.1016/j.brat.2011.10.011
MacKenzie, D. L., Wilson, D. B., & Kider, S. B. (2001). Effects of correctional boot camps on
offending. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 578, 126–143.
Madsen, M. V., Gøtzsche, P. C., & Hróbjartsson, A. (2009). Acupuncture treatment for pain:
Systematic review of randomised clinical trials with acupuncture, placebo acupuncture, and
no acupuncture groups. British Medical Journal, 338(7690). doi: 10.1136/bmj.a3115
Markowitz, J. C., Klerman, G. L., Clougherty, K. F., Spielman, L. A., Jacobsberg, L. B.,
Fishman, B., … Perry, S. W. (1995). Individual psychotherapies for depressed HIV-positive
patients. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 1504–1509.
Markowitz, J. C., Kocsis, J. H., Fishman, B., Spielman, L. A., Jacobsberg, L. B., Frances, A.
J., … Perry, S. W. (1998). Treatment of depressive symptoms in human immunodeficiency
virus-positive patients. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55(5), 452–457. doi:
10.1001/archpsyc.55.5.452
Markowitz, J. C., Manber, R., & Rosen, P. (2008). Therapists’ response to training in brief
supportive psychotherapy. American Journal of Psychotherapy, 62(1), 67–81.
Markowitz, J. C., Milrod, B., Bleiberg, K., & Marshall, R. D. (2009). Interpersonal factors in
understanding and treating posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Practice,
15(2), 133–140. doi: 10.1097/01.pra.0000348366.34419.28
Marlatt, G. A. (1983). The controlled-drinking controversy: A commentary. American
Psychologist, 38, 1097–1110.
Marlatt, G. A. (1985). Abstinence and controlled drinking: Alternative treatment goals for
alcoholism and problem drinking? Bulletin of the Society of Psychologists in Addictive
Behaviors, 4, 123–150.
Marlatt, G. A., & Gordon, J. (1985). Relapse prevention: Maintenance strategies in the
treatment of addictive behaviors. New York: Guilford.
Martin, D. J., Garske, J. P., & Davis, M. K. (2000). Relation of the therapeutic alliance with
outcome and other variables: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 68, 438–450.
Mattick, R. P., Andrews, G., Hadzi-Pavlovic, D., & Christensen, H. (1990). Treatment of panic
and agoraphobia: An integrative review. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 178(9),
567–576.
Mavissakalian, M., & Michelson, L. (1986). Agoraphobia: Relative and combined
effectiveness of therapist-assisted in vivo exposure and imipramine. Journal of Clinical
Psychiatry, 47, 117–122.
Mays, V. M., & Albee, G. W. (1992). Psychotherapy and ethnic minorities. In D. K.Freedheim
(Ed.), History of psychotherapy: A century of change (pp. 552–570). Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association.
McCall, W. A. (1923). How to experiment in education. New York: Macmillan.
McCullough, L., & Magill, M. (2009). Affect-focused short-term dynamic therapy. In R. A.Levy
& J. S.Ablon (Eds.), Handbook of evidence-based psychodynamic psychotherapy: Bridging
the gap between science and practice. (pp. 249–277). Totowa, NJ: Humana Press.
McDonagh, A., Friedman, M., McHugo, G., Ford, J., Sengupta, A., Mueser, K., … Descamps,
M. (2005). Randomized trial of cognitive-behavioral therapy for chronic posttraumatic stress
disorder in adult female survivors of childhood sexual abuse. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 73, 515–524.
McHugh, R. K., & Barlow, D. H. (2012). Dissemination and implementation of evidence-based
psychological interventions: Current status and future directions. In R. K.McHugh & D.
H.Barlow (Eds.), Dissemination and implementation of evidence-based psychological
interventions (pp. 247–263). New York: Oxford University Press.
McKay, K. M., Imel, Z. E., & Wampold, B. E. (2006). Psychiatrist effects in the
psychopharmacological treatment of depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 92(2),
287–290.
McLaughlin, A. A., Keller, S. M., Feeny, N. C., Youngstrom, E. A., & Zoellner, L. A. (2014).
Patterns of therapeutic alliance: Rupture–repair episodes in prolonged exposure for
posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 82(1), 112.
McNally, R. J. (1999). EMDR and Mesmerism: A comparative historical analysis. Journal of
Anxiety Disorders, 13, 225–236.
McNally, R. J., Bryant, R. A., & Ehlers, A. (2003). Does early psychological intervention
promote recovery from posttraumatic stress? Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 4,
45–79.
McNamara, K., & Horan, J.J. (1986). Experimental construct validity in the evaluation of
cognitive and behavioral treatments for depression. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 33:
23–30. doi:10.1037//0022-0167.33.1.23
Meehl, P. E. (1967). Theory-testing in psychology and physics: A methodological paradox.
Philosophy of Science, 34, 103–115.
Meehl, P. E. (1978). Theoretical risks and tabular asterisks: Sir Karl, Sir Ronald, and the slow
progress of soft psychology. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 46(4), 806–834.
doi: 10.1037/0022-006x.46.4.806
Meichenbaum, D. (1986). Cognitive-behavior modification. In F. H.Kanfer & A. P.Goldstein
(Eds.), Helping people change: A textbook of methods (3rd ed., pp. 346–380). New York:
Pergamon Press.
Meltzoff, J., and Kornreich, M. (1970). Research in psychotherapy. Chicago: Adline
Mercer, J. (2002). Attachment therapy: A treatment without empirical support. The Scientific
Review of Mental Health Practice, 1, 105–112.
Merrill, K. A., Tolbert, V. E., & Wade, W. A. (2003). Effectiveness of cognitive therapy for
depression in a community mental health center: A benchmarking study. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71(2), 404–409. doi: 10.1037/0022-006x.71.2.404
Mesmer, F. A. (1980). Mesmerism: A translation of the original scientific and medical writings
of E. A. Mesmer (G.Bloch, Trans.). Los Altos, CA: William Kaufman. (Original work published
1766)
Milgrom, J., Negri, L. M., Gemmill, A. W., McNeil, M., Martin, P. R. (2005). A randomized
controlled trial of psychological interventions for postnatal depression. British Journal of
Clinical Psychology, 44: 529–542. doi:10.1348/014466505x34200
Miller, D. (1994). Critical rationalism: A restatement and defense. Chicago: Open Court.
Miller, G. A. (1996). How we think about cognition, emotion, and biology in psychopathology.
Psychophysiology, 33, 615–628.
Miller, S. D., Duncan, B. L., Sorrell, R., & Brown, G. S. (2005). The Partners for Change
Outcome Management System. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 61(2), 199–208. doi:
10.1002/jclp.20111
Miller, W. R., Andrews, N. R., Wilbourne, P., & Bennett, M. E. (1998). A wealth of
alternatives: Effective treatments for alcohol problems. In W. R.Miller, & N.Heather (Eds.),
Treating addictive behaviors (2nd ed., pp. 203–216). New York: Plenum Press.
Miller, W. R., & Rollnick, S. (2002). Motivational interviewing (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford.
Miller, W. R., & Rollnick, S. (2009). Ten things that motivational interviewing is not.
Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 37(2), 129–140. doi:
10.1017/s1352465809005128
Miller, W. R., & Rollnick, S. (2012). Motivational interviewing (3rd ed.). New York: Guilford
Press.
Miller, W. R., & Rose, G. S. (2009). Toward a theory of motivational interviewing. American
Psychologist, 64, 527–537.
Mills, K. C., Sobell, M. B., & Schaefer, H. H. (1971). Training social drinking as an alternative
to abstinence for alcoholics. Behavior Therapy, 2, 18–27.
Minami, T., & Wampold, B. E. (2008). Adult psychotherapy in the real world. In W. B.Walsh
(Ed.), Biennial Review of Counseling Psychology (Vol. I, pp. 27–45). New York: Taylor and
Francis.
Minami, T., Davies, D. R., Tierney, S. C., Bettmann, J. E., McAward, S. M., Averill, L. A., …
Wampold, B. E. (2009). Preliminary evidence on the effectiveness of psychological
treatments delivered at a university counseling center. Journal of Counseling Psychology,
56(2), 309–320. doi: 10.1037/a0015398
Minami, T., Serlin, R. C., Wampold, B. E., Kircher, J., & Brown, G. S. (2008). Using clinical
trials to benchmark effects produced in clinical practice. Quality and Quantity, 42, 513–525.
Minami, T., Wampold, B. E., Serlin, R. C., Hamilton, E., Brown, G. S., & Kircher, J. (2008).
Benchmarking the effectiveness of psychotherapy treatment for adult depression in a
managed care environment: A preliminary study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 76, 116–124.
Minami, T., Wampold, B. E., Serlin, R. C., Kircher, J. C., & Brown, G. S. J. (2007).
Benchmarks for psychotherapy efficacy in adult major depression. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 75(2), 232–243.
Mohr, D.C. (1995). Negative outcome in psychotherapy: A critical review. Clinical
Psychology: Science and Practice, 2, 1–27.
Mohr, D. C., Beutler, L. E., Engle, D., Shoham-Salomon, V., Bergan, J., Kaszniak, A. W.,
(1990). Identification of patients at risk for nonresponse and negative outcome in
psychotherapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 58, 622–628.
Mohr, D. C., Spring, B., Freedland, K. E., Beckner, V., Arean, P., Hollon, S. D., … & Kaplan,
R. (2009). The selection and design of control conditions for randomized controlled trials of
psychological interventions. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 78(5), 275–284.
Molden, D. C., & Dweck, C. S. (2006). Finding “Meaning” in Psychology: A Lay Theories
Approach to Self-Regulation, Social Perception, and Social Development. American
Psychologist, 61(3), 192–203. doi: 10.1037/0003-066x.61.3.192
Montgomery, G. H., & Kirsch, I. (1997). Classical conditioning and the placebo effect. Pain,
72, 107–113.
Moos, R. H. (2005). Iatrogenic effects of psychosocial interventions for substance use
disorders: Prevalence, predictors, prevention. Addiction, 100, 595–604.
Morris, D. B. (1997). Placebo, pain, and belief: A biocultural model. In A.Harrington (Ed.), The
placebo effect: An interdisciplinary exploration (pp. 187–207). Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
Morris, D. B. (1998). Illness and culture in the postmodern age. Berkeley: University of
California Press.
Moses, E. B., & Barlow, D. H. (2006). A new unified treatment approach for emotional
disorders based on emotion science. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15,
146–150.
Moyers, T. B., & Miller, W. R. (2013). Is low therapist empathy toxic? Psychology of Addictive
Behaviors, 27(3), 878–884. doi: 10.1037/a0030274
Moyers, T. B., Miller, W. R., & Hendrickson, S. M. L. (2005). How does motivational
interviewing work? Therapist interpersonal skill predicts client involvement within motivational
interviewing sessions. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73(4), 590–598. doi:
10.1037/0022-006x.73.4.590
Munder, T., Brütsch, O., Leonhart, R., Gerger, H., & Barth, J. (2013). Researcher allegiance
in psychotherapy outcome research: An overview of reviews. Clinical Psychology Review,
33(4), 501–511. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2013.02.002
Munder, T., Gerger, H., Trelle, S., & Barth, J. (2011). Testing the allegiance bias hypothesis:
A meta-analysis. Psychotherapy Research, 21, 670–684.
Munder, T., Flückiger, C., Gerger, H., Wampold, B. E. & Barth, J. (2012). Is the allegiance
effect an epiphenomenon of true efficacy differences between treatments? A meta-analysis.
Journal of Counseling Psychology, 59, 632–637.
Nash, E., Hoehn-Sacric, R., Battle, C., Stone, A., Imber, S. D., & Frank, J. (1965). Systematic
preparation of patients for short-term psychotherapy: 2. Relation to characteristics of patient,
therapist, and the psychotherapeutic process. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disorders, 140,
374–383.
National Association of Cognitive-Behavioral Therapists (NACBT) (2014). Retrieved April 21,
2014, from www.nacbt.org/whatiscbt.aspx
National Collaborating Centre for Mental Health (2005). Post-traumatic stress disorder: The
management of PTSD in adults and children in primary and secondary care. London, Royal
College of Psychiatrists and Leicester, The British Psychological Society.
Neimeyer, R. A. (2000). Searching for the meaning of meaning: Grief therapy and the
process of reconstruction. Death Studies, 24, 541–558.
Niedenthal, P. M., & Brauer, M. (2012). Social functionality of human emotion. Annual Review
of Psychology, 63, 259–285. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.121208.131605
Nissen-Lie, H. A., Monsen, J. T., & Rønnestad, M. H. (2010). Therapist predictors of early
patient-rated working alliance: A multilevel approach. Psychotherapy Research, 20(6),
627–646. doi: 10.1080/10503307.2010.497633
Nitschke, J. B., Dixon, G. E., Sarinopoulos, I., Short, S. J., Cohen, J. D., Smith, E. E., …
Davidson, R. J. (2006). Altering expectancy dampens neural response to aversive taste in
primary taste cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 9(3), 435–442.
Norcross, J. C. (2011). Psychotherapy relationships that work: Evidence-based
responsiveness. New York: Oxford University Press.
Norcross, J. C., & Goldfried, M. R. (1992). Handbook of psychotherapy integration. New
York: Basic Books.
Norcross, J. C., & Goldfried, M. R. (2005). Handbook of psychotherapy integration (2nd ed.).
New York: Oxford University Press.
Norcross, J. C., & Karpiak, C. P. (2012). Clinical psychologists in the 2010s: 50 years of the
APA division of clinical psychology. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 19(1), 1–12.
doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2850.2012.01269.x
Norcross, J. C., Karpiak, C. P., & Santoro, S. O. (2005). Clinical psychologists across the
years: The division of clinical psychology from 1960 to 2003. Journal of Clinical Psychology,
61(12), 1467–1483. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20135
Norcross, J. C., & Newman, C. F. (1992). Psychotherapy integration: Setting the context. In J.
C.Norcross & M. R.Goldfried (Eds.), Handbook of psychotherapy integration (pp. 3–45). New
York: Basic Books.
Nowinski J, Baker S, & Carroll, K. (1992). Twelve step facilitation therapy manual: A clinical
research guide for therapists treating individuals with alcohol abuse and dependence.
Rockville, MD: NIAA.
Oei, T.P.S., & Free, M. L. (1995). Do cognitive behaviour therapies validate cognitive models
of mood disorders? A review of the empirical evidence. International Journal of Psychology,
30, 145–179.
Okiishi, J., Lambert, M. J., Nielsen, S. L., & Ogles, B. M. (2003). Waiting for super-shrink: An
empirical analysis of therapist effects. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 10(6), 361–373.
Orlinsky, D. E., & Howard, K. I. (1986). Process and outcome in psychotherapy. In S.
L.Garfield & A. E.Bergin (Eds.), Handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change (3rd ed.,
pp. 311–381). New York: Wiley.
Öst, L.G. (1987). Applied relaxation: Description of a coping technique and review of
controlled studies. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 25, 397–409.
Ougrin, D. (2011). Efficacy of exposure versus cognitive therapy in anxiety disorders:
systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC psychiatry, 11(1), 200.
Owen, J., & Hilsenroth, M. J. (2014). Treatment adherence: The importance of therapist
flexibility in relation to therapy outcomes. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 61, 280-288.
Papakostas, Y. G., & Daras, M. D. (2001). Placebos, placebo effect, and the response to the
healing situation: The evolution of a concept. Epilepsia, 42(12), 1614–1625.
Parloff, M. B. (1986). Frank’s “Common elements” in psychotherapy: Nonspecific factors and
placebos. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 56, 521–529.
Pattie, F. A. (1994). Mesmer and animal magnetism: A chapter in the history of medicine.
Hamilton, NY: Edmonston.
Paul, G. L. (1969). Behavior modification research: Design and tactics. In C. M.Franks (Ed.),
Behavior therapy: Appraisal and status (pp. 29–62). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Pendery, M. L., Maltzman, I. M., & West, L. J. (1982). Controlled drinking by alcoholics? New
findings and a reevaluation of a major affirmative study. Science, 217, 169–175.
Perepletchikova, F. (2009). Treatment integrity and differential treatment effects. Clinical
Psychology: Science and Practice, 16(3), 379–382. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2850.2009.01177.x
Persons, J. B., & Silberschatz, G. (1998). Are results of randomized controlled trials useful to
psychotherapists? Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 126–135.
Petrosino, A., Turpin-Petrosino, C., & Buehler, J. (2003). Scared Straight and other juvenile
awareness programs for preventing juvenile delinquency: A systematic review of the
randomized experimental evidence. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social
Science, 589, 41–62.
Phillips, E.L. (1957). Psychotherapy: A modern theory and practice. London: Staples.
Pilkonis, P. A., Imber, S. D., Lewis, P., & Rubinsky, P. (1984). A comparative outcome study
of individual, group, and conjoint psychotherapy. Archives of General Psychiatry, 41,
431–437.
Piper, W. E., Debbane, E. G., Bienvenu, J. P., & Garant, J. (1984). A comparative study of
four forms of psychotherapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 52(2), 268–279.
Pinsof, W. M., & Wynne, L. C. (2000). Toward progress research: Closing the gap between
family therapy practice and research. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 26(1), 1–8. doi:
10.1111/j.1752-0606.2000.tb00270.x
Pinsof, W. M., Zinbarg, R. E., Lebow, J. L., Knobloch-Fedders, L. M., Durbin, E., Chambers,
A., … Friedman, G. (2009). Laying the foundation for progress research in family, couple, and
individual therapy: The development and psychometric features of the initial Systemic
Therapy Inventory of Change. Psychotherapy Research, 19(2), 143–156. doi:
10.1080/10503300802669973
Piper, W. E., Debbane, E. G., Bienvenu, J. P., & Garant, J. (1984). A comparative study of
four forms of psychotherapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 52(2), 268–279.
Plassmann, H., O’Doherty, J., Shiv, B., & Rangel, A. (2008). Marketing actions can modulate
neural representations of experienced pleasantness. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences, 105(3), 1050–1054.
Pollo, A., Amanzio, M., Arslanian, A., Casadio, C., Maggi, G., & Benedetti, F. (2001).
Response expectancies in placebo analgesia and their clinical relevance. Pain, 93(1), 77–84.
doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(01)00296-2
Popper, K. R. (1963). Conjectures and refutations. London: Routledge.
Popper, K. R. (1962). On the sources of knowledge and of ignorance. Conjectures and
refutations: The growth of scientific knowledge. New York: Basic Books.
Popper, K. R. (1972). Objective knowledge: An evolutionary approach. Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
Porter, A. C., & Raudenbush, S. W. (1987). Analysis of covariance: Its model and use in
psychological research. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 34, 383–392.
Poulsen, S., Lunn, S., Daniel, S.I.F., Folke, S., Mathiesen, B. B., Katznelson, H., & Fairburn,
C. G. (2014). A randomized controlled trial of psychoanalytic psychotherapy or cognitive-
behavioral therapy for bulimia nervosa. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 171(1),
109–116. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.12121511
Powers, M. B., Halpern, J. M., Ferenschak, M. P., Gillihan, S. J., & Foa, E. B. (2010). A meta-
analytic review of prolonged exposure for posttraumatic stress disorder. Clinical Psychology
Review, 30, 635–641.
Powers, M. B., Smits, J.A.J., Whitley, D., Bystritsky, A., & Telch, M. J. (2008). The effect of
attributional processes concerning medication taking on return of fear. Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology, 76 (3), 478–490.
Preston, S. D., & de Waal, F.B.M. (2002). Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases.
Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 1–20.
Price, D. P., Finniss, D. G., & Benedetti, F. (2008). A comprehensive review of the placebo
effect: Recent advances and current thought. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 565–590.
Prochaska, J. O., & Norcross, J. C. (2002). Stages of change. In J. C.Norcross (Ed.),
Psychotherapy relationships that work: Therapist contributions and responsiveness to
patients (pp. 303–313). New York: Oxford University.
Project MATCH Research Group. (1997). Matching alcoholism treatments to client
heterogeneity: Project MATCH Posttreatment drinking outcomes . Journal of Studies on
Alcohol, 58, 7–29.
Project MATCH Research Group. (1998). Therapist effects in three treatments for alcohol
problems. Psychotherapy Research, 8 (4), 455–474.
Propst, L. R., Ostrom, R., Watkins, P., Dean, T., & Mashburn, D. (1992). Comparative
efficacy of religious and nonreligious cognitive-behavioral therapy for the treatment of clinical
depression in religious individuals. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 60,
94–103.
Puschner, B., Wolf, M., & Kraft, S. (2008). Helping alliance and outcome in psychotherapy:
What predicts what in routine outpatient treatment? Psychotherapy Research, 18 (2),
167–178. doi: 10.1080/10503300701367984
Rachman, S. (1971). The effects of psychotherapy (Vol. 15). Oxford: Pergamon.
Rachman, S. (1977). Double standards and single standards. Bulletin of the British
Psychological Society, 30 (AUG), 295–295.
Rachman, S., & Wilson, G. T. (1980). The effects of psychological therapy. Oxford:
Pergamon Press.
Ramseyer, F., & Tschacher, W. (2011). Nonverbal synchrony in psychotherapy: Coordinated
body movement reflects relationship quality and outcome. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 79, 284–295.
Raudenbush, S. W. (2009). Analyzing effect sizes: Random-effects models. In H.Cooper, L.
V.Hedges & J. C.Valentine (Eds.), The handbook of research synthesis and meta-analysis
(2nd ed., pp. 295–316). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.
Rhule, D. M. (2005). Take care to do no harm: Harmful interventions for youth problem
behavior. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 36, 618–625.
Rice, L. N., & Greenberg, L. S. (1992). Humanistic approaches to psychotherapy. In D.
K.Freedheim (Ed.), History of psychotherapy: A century of change (pp. 197–224).
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Robinson, L. A., Berman, J. S., & Neimeyer, R. A. (1990). Psychotherapy for the treatment of
depression: A comprehensive review of controlled outcome research. Psychological Bulletin,
108, 30–49.
Rogers, C. R. (1951a). Client-centered therapy. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Rogers, C. R. (1951b). A research program in client-centered therapy. Research
publications—Association for Research in Nervous and Mental Disorders, 31, 106–113.
Rosa-Alcázar, A.I., Sánchez-Meca, J., Gómez-Conesa, A., & Marín-Martínez, F. (2008).
Psychological treatment of obsessive–compulsive disorder: A meta-analysis. Clinical
Psychology Review, 28, 1310–1325.
Rose, S., Bisson, J., & Wessely, S. (2001). Psychological debriefing for preventing post
traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). (Cochrane Library, Issue 3.) Oxford: Update Software.
Rosen, G. M. (1999). Treatment fidelity and research on Eye Movement Desensitization and
Reprocessing (EMDR). Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 13, 173–184.
Rosenbaum, D. P., & Hanson, G. S. (1998). Assessing the effects of school-based drug
education: A six-year multilevel analysis of Project D.A.R.E. Journal of Research in Crime
and Delinquency, 35 (4), 381–412.
Rosenquist, J. N., Fowler, J. H., & Christakis, N. A. (2011). Social network determinants of
depression. Molecular Psychiatry, 16 (3), 273–281. doi: 10.1038/mp.2010.13
Rosenthal, D., & Frank, J. D. (1956). Psychotherapy and the placebo effect. Psychological
Bulletin, 53, 294–302.
Rosenthal, R. (1994). Parametric measures of effect size. In H.Cooper & L. V.Hedges (Eds.),
The handbook of research synthesis (pp. 231–260). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.
Rosenzweig, S. (1936). Some implicit common factors in diverse methods of psychotherapy:
“At last the Dodo said, ‘Everybody has won and all must have prizes’.” American Journal of
Orthopsychiatry, 6, 412–415.
Rosenzweig, S. (1954). A transvaluation of psychotherapy: a reply to Hans Eysenck. The
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 49, 298–304.
Roth, W. T., Wilhelm, F. H., & Petit, D. (2005). Are current theories of panic falsifiable?
Psychological Bulletin, 131, 171–192.
Rubin, D. B. (1986). Statistics and causal inference—which ifs have causal answers. Journal
of the American Statistical Association, 81 (396), 961–962. doi: 10.2307/2289065
Ruzek, J. I., Karlin, B. E., & Zeiss, A. (2012). Implementation of evidence-based
psychological treatments in the Veterans Health Administration. In R. K.McHugh & D.
H.Barlow (Eds.), Dissemination and implementation of evidence-based psychological
interventions (pp. 78–96). New York: Oxford University Press.
Sackett, D. L., Straus, S. E., Richardson, W. S., Rosenberg, W., & Haynes, R. B. (2000).
Evidence-based medicine: How to practice and teach EBM (2nd ed.). London: Churchill
Livingstone.
Safran, J. D., & Muran, J. C. (2000). Negotiating the therapeutic alliance. New York: Guilford.
Safran, J. D., Muran, J. C., & Eubanks-Carter, C. (2011). Repairing alliance ruptures.
Psychotherapy, 48 (1), 80–87. doi: 10.1037/a0022140
Sagan, C. In P. G.Blacketor (2009). Everyday useful quotes. Xlibris.
Sánchez-Meca, J., Rosa-Alcázar, A. I., Marín-Martínez, F., & Gómez-Conesa, A. (2010).
Psychological treatment of panic disorder with or without agoraphobia: a meta-analysis.
Clinical Psychology Review, 30 (1), 37–50.
Sapyta, J., Riemer, M., & Bickman, L. (2005). Feedback to Clinicians: Theory, Research, and
Practice. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 61 (2), 145–153. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20107
Saxon, D., & Barkham, M. (2012). Patterns of therapist variability: Therapist effects and the
contribution of patient severity and risk. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 80 (4),
535–546. doi: 10.1037/a0028898
Schneider Institute for Health Policy, Brandeis University for the Robert Wood Johnson
Foundation. (2001). Substance abuse: The nation’s number one health problem: Key
indicators for policy update. Princeton, NJ: The Robert Wood Johnson.
Schnurr, P. P., Friedman, M. J., Engel, C. C., Foa, E. B., Shea, M. T., Chow, B. K., (2007).
Cognitive behavioral therapy for posttraumatic stress disorder in women: A randomized
controlled trial. JAMA: Journal of the American Medical Association, 297, 820–830.
Schnurr, P. P., Shea, M. T., Friedman, M. J., & Engel, C. C. (2007). ‘Posttraumatic stress
disorder and cognitive behavioral therapy’: In reply. Journal of the American Medical
Association, 297 (24). doi: 10.1001/jama.297.24.2695
Seidler, G. H., & Wagner, F. E. (2006). Comparing the efficacy of EMDR and trauma-focused
cognitive-behavioral therapy in the treatment of PTSD: a meta-analytic study. Psychological
Medicine, 36 (11), 1515–1522.
Seligman, M. E. (1995). The effectiveness of psychotherapy: The Consumer Reports study.
American Psychologist, 50 (12), 965–974.
Serlin, R. C., & Lapsley, D. K. (1985). Rationality in psychological research: The good-
enough principle. American Psychologist, 40 (1), 73–83. doi: 10.1037/0003-066x. 40.1.73
Serlin, R. C., & Lapsley, D. K. (1993). Rational appraisal of psychological research and the
good-enough principle. In G.Keren, C.Lewis, G.Keren & C.Lewis (Eds.), A handbook for data
analysis in the behavioral sciences: Methodological issues (pp. 199–228). Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Serlin, R. C., Wampold, B. E., & Levin, J. R. (2003). Should providers of treatment be
regarded as a random factor? If it ain’t broke, don’t “Fix” it: A comment on Siemer and
Joorman (2003). Psychological Methods, 8, 524–534.
Shadish, W. R., & Haddock, C. K. (2009). Combining estimates of effect size. In H.Cooper, L.
V.Hedges & J. C.Valentine (Eds.), The handbook of research synthesis and meta-analysis
(2nd ed., pp. 257–277). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.
Shadish, W. R., Matt, G.E., Navarro, A. M., & Phillips, G. (2000). The effects of psychological
therapies in clinically representative conditions: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 126,
512–529.
Shadish, W. R., Matt, G. E., Navarro, A. M., Siegle, G., Crits-Christoph, P., Hazelrigg, M. D.,
… Weiss, B. (1997). Evidence that therapy works in clinically representative conditions.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65, 355–365.
Shadish, W. R., Montgomery, L. M., Wilson, P., Wilson, M. R., Bright, I., & Okwumabua, T.
(1993). Effects of family and marital psychotherapies: A meta-analysis. Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology, 61 (6), 992.
Shadish, W. R., & Sweeney, R. B. (1991). Mediators and moderators in meta-analysis:
There’s a reason we don’t let dodo birds tell us which psychotherapies should have prizes.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59 (6), 883–893
Shafran, R., Clark, D. M., Fairburn, C. G., Arntz, A., Barlow, D. H., Ehlers, A., … Wilson, G.
T. (2009). Mind the gap: Improving the dissemination of CBT. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 47 (11), 902–909. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2009.07.003
Shapiro, D. A., Barkham, M., Rees, A., Hardy, G. E., Reynolds, S., & Startup, M. (1994).
Effects of treatment duration and severity of depression on the effectiveness of cognitive-
behavioral and psychodynamic-interpersonal psychotherapy. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 62, 522–534.
Shapiro, A. K., & Morris, L. A. (1978). The placebo effect in medical and psychological
therapies. In S. L.Garfield & A. E.Bergin (Eds.), Handbook of psychotherapy and behavior
change (2nd ed., pp. 369–410). New York: Wiley.
Shapiro, A. K., & Shapiro, E. S. (1997a). The placebo: Is it much ado about nothing? In
A.Harrington (Ed.), The placebo effect: An interdisciplinary exploration. Cambridge, MA:
Harvard University Press.
Shapiro, A. K., & Shapiro, E. S. (1997b). The powerful placebo: From ancient priest to
modern medicine. Baltimore: The Johns Hopkins University Press.
Shapiro, D. A., & Shapiro, D. (1982). Meta-analysis of comparative therapy outcome studies:
A replication and refinement. Psychological Bulletin, 92, 581–604.
Shaw, B. F., Elkin, I., Yamaguchi, J., Olmsted, M., Vallis, T. M., Dobson, K. S., … Imber, S.
D. (1999). Therapist competence ratings in relation to clinical outcome in cognitive therapy of
depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 67, 837–846.
Shedler, J. (2010). The efficacy of psychodynamic psychotherapy. American Psychologist, 65
(2), 98–109. doi: 10.1037/a0018378
Shepherd, M. (1993). The placebo: From specificity to the non-specific and back.
Psychological Medicine, 23 (3), 569–578.
Sherman, J. J. (1998). Effects of psychotherapeutic treatments for PTSD: A meta-analysis of
controlled clinical trial. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 11, 413–435.
Shimokawa, K., Lambert, M. J., & Smart, D. W. (2010). Enhancing treatment outcome of
patients at risk of treatment failure: Meta-analytic and mega-analytic review of a
psychotherapy quality assurance system. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 78
(3), 298–311. doi: 10.1037/a0019247
Shirk, S. R., Karver, M. S., & Brown, R. (2011). The alliance in child and adolescent
psychotherapy. Psychotherapy, 48 (1), 17–24. doi: 10.1037/a0022181
Siev, J., & Chambless, D. L. (2007). Specificity of treatment effects: Cognitive therapy and
relaxation for generalized anxiety and panic disorders. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 75, 513–522.
Siev, J., Huppert, J. D., & Chambless, D. L. (2009). The dodo bird, treatment technique, and
disseminating empirically supported treatments. The Behavior Therapist, 32, 69–76.
Simon, G., Imel, Z. E., & Steinfield, B. J. (2012). Is dropout after a first psychotherapy visit
always a bad outcome? Psychiatric Services, 63, 705–707.
Simon G.E. & Ludman, E.J. (2010). Predictors of early dropout from psychotherapy for
depression in community practice. Psychiatric Services, 61, 684–689.
Simpson, S. H., Eurich, D. T., Majumdar, S. R., Padwal, R. S., Tsuyuki, S. T., Varney, J., &
Johnson, J. A. (2006). A meta-analysis of the association between adherence to drug therapy
and mortality. British Medical Journal, 3–4. doi:10.1136/bmj.38875.675486.55.
Singer, M. T., & Lalich, J. (1996). “Crazy” therapies: What are they? Do they work? New
York: Jossey-Bass.
Sloane, R. B., Staples, F. R., Cristol, A. H., Yorkston, N. J., & Whipple, K. (1975).
Psychotherapy versus behavior therapy. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Smith, B., & Sechrest, L. (1991). Treatment of Aptitude × Treatment Interactions. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59 (2), 233–244. doi: 10.1037/0022-006x. 59.2.233
Smith, M. L., & Glass, G. V. (1977). Meta-analysis of psychotherapy outcome studies.
American Psychologist, 32, 752–760.
Smith, M. L., Glass, G. V., & Miller, T. I. (1980). The benefits of psychotherapy. Baltimore:
The Johns Hopkins University Press.
Snijders, T., & Bosker, R. (1999). Multilevel analysis: An introduction to basic and advanced
multilevel modeling. London: Sage.
Snyder, D. K., & Wills, R. M. (1989). Behavioral versus insight-oriented marital therapy:
Effects on individual and interpersonal functioning. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 57, 39–46.
Snyder, D. K., & Wills, R. M. (1991). Facilitating change in marital therapy and research.
Journal of Family Psychology, 4, 426–435.
Snyder, D. K., Wills, R. M., & Grady-Fletcher, A. (1991). Long term effectiveness of
behavioral versus insight oriented marital therapy: A 4-year follow-up study. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59, 138–141.
Sobell, L. C., Sobell, M. B., & Christelman, W. C. (1972). The myth of “one drink.” Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 10, 119–123.
Sobell, M.B. & Sobell, L.C. (1973). Alcoholics treated by individualized behavior therapy: One
year treatment outcomes. Behavior Research and Therapy, 11, 599–618.
Sobell, M. B., & Sobell, L. C. (1976). Second year treatment outcome of alcoholics treated by
individualized behavior therapy: Results. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 14, 195–215.
Sobell, M. B., & Sobell, L. C. (1984a). The aftermath of heresy: A response to Pendery et
al.’s (1982) critique of ‘Individualized behavior therapy for alcoholics’. Behaviour Research
and Therapy, 22, 413–440.
Sobell, M. B., & Sobell, L. C. (1984b). Under the microscope yet again: A commentary on
walker and roach’s critique of the dickens committee’s enquiry into our research. British
Journal of Addiction, 79, 157–168.
Society of Clinical Psychology. (2007). Website on Research Supported Psychological
Treatments, www.div12.org/PsychologicalTreatments/index.html
Spiegel, A. (2004). Cognitive behavior therapy: Thinking positive [Radio series episode]. “All
Things Considered.” Washington, DC: National Public Radio. Retrieved from
www.npr.org/templates/story/story.php?storyId=1920052
Spielmans, G. I., Gatlin, E. T., & McFall, J. P. (2010). The efficacy of evidence-based
psychotherapies versus usual care for youths: Controlling confounds in a meta-reanalysis.
Psychotherapy Research, 20 (2), 234–246. doi: 10.1080/10503300903311293
Spielmans, G. I., & Kirsch, I. (in press). Drug approval and drug effectiveness. Annual Review
of Clinical Psychology. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212–185533
Spielmans, G. I., Pasek, L. F., & McFall, J. P. (2007). What are the active ingredients in
cognitive and behavioral psychotherapy for anxious and depressed children? A meta-analytic
review. Clinical Psychology Review, 27 (5), 642–654. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2006.06.001
Stangier, U., Schramm, E., Heidenreich, T., Berger, M., & Clark, D. M. (2011). Cognitive
therapy vs interpersonal psychotherapy in social anxiety disorder: a randomized controlled
trial. Archives of General Psychiatry, 68 (7), 692–700.
Stevens, S. E., Hynan, M. T., & Allen, M. (2000). A meta-analysis of common factor and
specific treatment effects across domains of the phase model of psychotherapy. Clinical
Psychology: Science and Practice, 7, 273–290.
Stiles, W. B., Shapiro, D. A., & Elliott, R. (1986). Are all psychotherapies equivalent?
American Psychologist, 41, 165–180.
Strunk, D. R., Brotman, M. A., & DeRubeis, R. J. (2010). The process of change in cognitive
therapy for depression: Predictors of early inter-session symptom gains. Behaviour Research
and Therapy, 48 (7), 599–606. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2010.03.011
Strunk, D. R., Cooper, A. A., Ryan, E. T., DeRubeis, R. J., & Hollon, S. D. (2012). The
process of change in cognitive therapy for depression when combined with antidepressant
medication: Predictors of early intersession symptom gains. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 80 (5), 730–738. doi: 10.1037/a0029281
Strupp, H. H., & Howard, K. I. (1992). A brief history of psychotherapy research. In D.
K.Freedheim (Ed.), History of psychotherapy: A century of change (pp. 309–334).
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Sullivan, H. S. (1953). The interpersonal theory of psychiatry. New York: Routledge.
Surgeon General. (1999). Mental Health: A Report of the Surgeon General—Executive
Summary. Rockville, MD: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Substance Abuse
and Mental Health Services Administration, Center for Mental Health Services, National
Institutes of Health, National Institute of Mental Health.
Surís, A., Link-Malcolm, J., Chard, K., Ahn, C., & North, C. (2013). A randomized clinical trial
of cognitive processing therapy for veterans with PTSD related to military sexual trauma.
Journal of Traumatic Stress, 26 (1), 28–37. doi: 10.1002/jts.21765
Sutton, A. J. (2009). Publication bias. In H.Cooper, L. V.Hedges & J. C.Valentine (Eds.), The
hanbook of research synthesis and meta-analysis (2nd ed., pp. 435–454). New York: Russell
Sage Foundation.
Swift, J. K., Callahan, J. L., & Vollmer, B. M. (2011). Preferences. Journal of Clinical
Psychology, 67 (2), 155–165. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20759
Swift, J. K., & Greenberg, R. P. (2012). Premature discontinuation in adult psychotherapy: A
meta-analysis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 80 (4), 547–559. doi:
10.1037/a0028226
Tang, T. Z., & DeRubeis, R. J. (1999). Reconsidering rapid early response in cognitive
behavioral therapy for depression. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 6 (3), 283–288.
doi: 10.1093/clipsy/6.3.283
Tarrier, N., Pilgrim, H., Sommerfield, C., Faragher, B., Reynolds, M., Graham, E., &
Barrowclough, C. (1999). A randomized trial of cognitive therapy and imaginal exposure in
the treatment of chronic posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 67, 13–18.
Tasca, G. A., & Lampard, A.M. (2012). Reciprocal influence of alliance to the group and
outcome in day treatment for eating disorders. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 59 (4),
507–517. doi: 10.1037/a0029947
Task Force onPromotion and Dissemination of PsychologicalProcedures. (1995). Training in
and dissemination of empirically-validated psychological treatment: Report and
recommendations. The Clinical Psychologist, 48, 2–23.
Taylor, E. (1999). Shadow culture: Psychology and spirituality in America. Washington, DC:
Counterpoint.
Taylor, S. (1996). Meta-analysis of cognitive-behavioral treatments for social phobia. Journal
of Behaviour Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 27, 1–9.
Taylor, S., Thordarson, D. S., Maxfield, L., Fedoroff, I. C., Lovell, K., & Ogrodniczuk, J.
(2003). Comparative efficacy, speed, and adverse effects of three PTSD treatments:
Exposure therapy, EMDR, and relaxation training. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 71 (2), 330–338. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.71.2.330
Thomas, R. M. (2001). Folk psychologies across cultures. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Tolin, D. F. (2010). Is cognitive–behavioral therapy more effective than other therapies?: A
meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30 (6), 710–720. doi:
10.1016/j.cpr.2010.05.003
Tracey, T.J.G., Wampold, B. E., Lichtenberg, J. W., & Goodyear, R. K. (2014). Expertise in
Psychotherapy: An Elusive Goal? American Psychologist. doi: 10.1037/a0035099
Truax, C. B. (1966). Reinforcement and nonreinforcement in Rogerian psychotherapy.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 71 (1), 1–9. doi: 10.1037/h0022912
Tryon, G. S., & Winograd, G. (2011). Goal consensus and collaboration. In J. C.Norcross
(Ed.), Psychotherapy relationships that work: Evidence-based responsiveness (2nd ed., pp.
153–167). New York: Oxford University Press.
UKATT Research Team. (2007). UK alcohol treatment trial: client-treatment matching effects.
Addiction, 103, 228–238.
Ulvenes, P. G., Berggraf, L., Hoffart, A., Stiles, T. C., Svartberg, M., McCullough, L., &
Wampold, B. E. (2012). Different processes for different therapies: Therapist actions,
therapeutic bond, and outcome. Psychotherapy, 49 (3), 291–302. doi: 10.1037/a0027895
van Balkom, A. J., van Oppen, P., Vermeulen, A. W., van Dyck, R., Nauta, M. C., & Vorst, H.
(1994). A meta-analysis on the treatment of obsessive compulsive disorder: a comparison of
antidepressants, behavior, and cognitive therapy. Clinical Psychology Review, 14 (5),
359–381.
van Emmerik, A.A.P., Kamphuis, J. H., Hulsbosch, A. M., & Emmelkamp, P.M.G. (2002).
Single session debriefing after psychological trauma: A meta-analysis. The Lancet, 360,
766–771.
van Minnen, A., & Foa, E. B. (2006). The Effect of Imaginal Exposure Length on Outcome of
Treatment for PTSD. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 19 (4), 427–438. doi: 10.1002/jts.20146
VandenBos, G. R., Cummings, N. A., & DeLeon, P. H. (1992). A century of psychotherapy:
Economic and environmental influences. In D. K.Freedheim (Ed.), History of psychotherapy:
A century of change (pp. 65–102). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Vase, L., Riley III, J. L., & Price, D. P. (2002). A comparison of placebo effects in clinical
analgesic trials versus studies of placebo analgesia. Pain, 99, 443–452.
Vollmer, S., Spada, H., Caspar, F., & Burri, S. (2013). Expertise in clinical psychology. The
effects of university training and practical experience on expertise in clinical psychology.
Frontiers in Psychology, 4, article 141.
Vos, S.P.F., Huibers, M.J.H., Diels, L., & Arntz, A. (2012). A randomized clinical trial of
cognitive behavioral therapy and interpersonal psychotherapy for panic disorder with
agoraphobia. Psychological Medicine, 42 (12), 2661–2672.
doi:10.1017/S0033291712000876
Wachtel, P. L. (1977). Psychoanalysis and behavior therapy: Toward an integration. New
York: Basic Books.
Wade, W. A., Treat, T. A., & Stuart, G. L. (1998). Transporting an empircally supported
treatment for panic disorder to a service clinic setting: A benchmarking strategy. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 231–239.
Walach, H. (2003). Placebo and placebo effects—a concise review. Focus on Alternative and
Complementary Therapies, 8, 178–187.
Walsh, J. E. (1947). Concerning the effect of intraclass correlation on certain significance
tests. The Annals of Mathematical Statistics, 18 (1), 88–96.
Walsh, R. (2011). Lifestyle and mental health. American Psychologist, 66 (7), 579–592. doi:
10.1037/a0021769
Waltz, J., Addis, M. E., Koerner, K., & Jacobson, N. S. (1993). Testing the integrity of a
psychotherapy protocol: Assessment of adherence and competence. Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology, 61, 620–630.
Wampold, B. E. (1997). Methodological problems in identifying efficacious psychotherapies.
Psychotherapy Research, 7, 21–43.
Wampold, B. E. (2001a). Contextualizing psychotherapy as a healing practice: Culture,
history, and methods. Applied and Preventive Psychology, 10, 69–86.
Wampold, B. E. (2001b). The great psychotherapy debate: Model, methods, and findings.
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Wampold, B. E. (2007). Psychotherapy: The humanistic (and effective) treatment. American
Psychologist, 62, 857–873.
Wampold, B. E. (2013). The good, the bad, and the ugly: A 50-year perspective on the
outcome problem. Psychotherapy, 50 (1), 16–24. doi: 10.1037/a0030570
Wampold, B. E., & Bhati, K. S. (2004). Attending to the omissions: A historical examination of
the evidenced-based practice movement. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice,
35, 563–570.
Wampold, B. E., & Brown, G. S. (2005). Estimating therapist variability: A naturalistic study of
outcomes in managed care. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73, 914–923.
Wampold, B. E., & Budge, S. L. (2012). The 2011 Leona Tyler Award Address: The
Relationship—and Its Relationship to the Common and Specific Factors of Psychotherapy.
The Counseling Psychologist, 40 (4), 601–623. doi: 10.1177/0011000011432709
Wampold, B. E., Budge, S. L., Laska, K. M., Del Re, A. C., Baardseth, T. P., Flückiger, C., …
Gunn, W. (2011). Evidence-based treatments for depression and anxiety versus treatment-
as-usual: A meta-analysis of direct comparisons. Clinical Psychology Review, 31 (8),
1304–1312. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2011.07.012
Wampold, B. E., & Drew, C. J. (1990). Theory and application of statistics. New York:
McGraw-Hill College.
Wampold, B. E., Goodheart, C. D., & Levant, R. F. (2007). Evidence-based practice in
psychology: Clarification and elaboration. American Psychologist, 62, 616–618.
Wampold, B. E. & Imel, Z. E. (2006). Psychotherapy stories: A textbook case of privilege.
Review of the Oxford Textbook of Psychotherapy. Contemporary Psychology: APA Review of
Books, 51 (20).
Wampold, B. E., Imel, Z. E., Bhati, K. S., & Johnson Jennings, M. D. (2006). Insight as a
common factor. In L. G.Castonguay & C. E.Hill (Eds.), Insight in psychotherapy,
119–139.Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Wampold, B. E., Imel, Z. E., Laska, K. M., Benish, S., Miller, S. D., Flückiger, C., … Budge,
S. (2010). Determining what works in the treatment of PTSD. Clinical Psychology Review, 30
(8), 923–933. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.06.005
Wampold, B. E., Imel, Z. E., & Miller, S. D. (2009). Barriers to the dissemination of empirically
supported treatments: Matching messages to the evidence. The Behavior Therapist, 32 (7),
144–155.
Wampold, B. E., Imel, Z. E., & Minami, T. (2007a). The placebo effect: ‘Relatively large’ and
‘robust’ enough to survive another assault. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 63 (4), 401–403.
doi: 10.1002/jclp.20350
Wampold, B. E., Imel, Z. E., & Minami, T. (2007b). The story of placebo effects in medicine:
Evidence in context. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 63, 379–390.
Wampold, B. E., Minami, T., Baskin, T. W., & Callen Tierney, S. (2002). A meta-(re) analysis
of the effects of cognitive therapy versus ‘other therapies’ for depression. Journal of Affective
Disorders, 68 (2), 159–165.
Wampold, B. E., Minami, T., Tierney, S. C., Baskin, T. W., & Bhati, K. S. (2005). The placebo
is powerful: Estimating placebo effects in medicine and psychotherapy from clinical trials.
Journal of Clinical Psychology, 61, 835–854.
Wampold, B. E., Mondin, G. W., Moody, M., & Ahn, H. (1997a). The flat earth as a metaphor
for the evidence for uniform efficacy of bona fide psychotherapies: Reply to Crits-Christoph
(1997) and Howard et al. (1997). Psychological Bulletin, 122, 226–230.
Wampold, B. E., Mondin, G. W., Moody, M., Stich, F., Benson, K., & Ahn, H. (1997b). A
meta-analysis of outcome studies comparing bona fide psychotherapies: Empirically, “All
must have prizes.” Psychological Bulletin, 122, 203–215.
Wampold, B. E., & Serlin, R. C. (2000). The consequences of ignoring a nested factor on
measures of effect size in analysis of variance. Psychological Methods, 5, 425–433.
Wampold, B. E., & Serlin, R. C. (2014). Meta-analytic methods to test relative efficacy.
Quality and Quantity, 48, 755–765. doi: 10.1007/s11135-012-9800-6
Wang, P. S., Demler, O., Olfson, M., Pincus, H. A., Wells, K. B., & Kessler, R. C. (2006).
Changing profiles of service sectors used for mental health care in the United States.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 1187–1198.
Wang, P. S., Lane, M., Olfson, M., Pincus, H. A., Wells, K. B., & Kessler, R. C. (2005).
Twelve-month use of mental health services in the United States: Results from the National
Comorbidity Survey Replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, 629–640.
Watson, J. B., & Rayner, R. (1920). Conditioned emotional reactions. Experimental
psychology, 3, 1–14.
Watson, J. C., Gordon, L. B., Stermac, L., Kalogerakos, F., & Steckley, P. (2003). Comparing
the effectiveness of process-experiential with cognitive-behavioral psychotherapy in the
treatment of depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71, 773–781.
Webb, C. A., DeRubeis, R. J., Amsterdam, J. D., Shelton, R. C., Hollon, S. D., & Dimidjian, S.
(2011). Two aspects of the therapeutic alliance: Differential relations with depressive
symptom change. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 79 (3), 279–283. doi:
10.1037/a0023252
Webb, C. A., DeRubeis, R. J., & Barber, J. P. (2010). Therapist adherence/competence and
treatment outcome: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
78 (2), 200–211. doi: 10.1037/a0018912
Weersing, V. R., & Weisz, J. R. (2002). Community clinic treatment of depressed youth:
Benchmarking usual care against CBT clinical trials. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 70 (2), 299–310.
Weiss, B., Caron, A., Ball, S., Tapp, J., Johnson, M., & Weisz, J. R. (2005). Iatrogenic effects
of group treatment for antisocial youths. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73,
1036–1044.
Weissman, M. M. (2006). A Brief History of Interpersonal Psychotherapy. Psychiatric Annals,
36 (8), 553–557.
Weisz, J. R., Jensen-Doss, A., & Hawley, K. M. (2006). Evidence-based youth
psychotherapies versus usual clinical care: A meta-analysis of direct comparisons. American
Psychologist, 61 (7), 671–689. doi: 10.1037/0003-066x.61.7.671
Werch, C. E., & Owen, D. M. (2002). Iatrogenic effects of alcohol and drug prevention
programs. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 63, 581–590.
West, S. L., & O’Neal, K. K. (2004). Project D.A.R.E. outcome effectiveness revisited.
American Journal of Public Health, 94, 1027–1029.
Westen, D. (1998). The scientific legacy of Sigmund Freud: Toward a psychodynamically
informed psychological science. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 333–371.
Westen, D., & Bradley, R. (2005). Empirically Supported Complexity. Rethinking Evidence-
Based Practice in Psychotherapy. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14 (5),
266–271.
Westen, D., Novotny, C. M., & Thompson-Brenner, H. (2004). The empirical status of
empirically supported psychotherapies: assumptions, findings, and reporting in controlled
clinical trials. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 631–663.
Westen, D., Novotny, C. M., & Thompson-Brenner, H. (2005). EBP =/EST: Reply to Crits-
Christoph et al. (2005) and Weisz et al. (2005). Psychological Bulletin, 131 (3), 427–433.
White, W. L. (1998). Slaying the dragon: The history of addiction treatment and recovery in
America. Bloomington, IL: Chestnut Health Systems.
Wilkins, W. (1983). Failure of placebo groups to control for nonspecific events in therapy
outcome research. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 20, 31–37.
Wilkins, W. (1984). Psychotherapy: The powerful placebo. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 52, 570–573.
Williams, A.C.d.C. (2002). Facial expression of pain: An evolutionary account. Behavioral and
Brain Sciences, 25 (4), 439–488. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x02000080
Willis, J., & Todorov, A. (2006). First Impressions: Making Up Your Mind After a 100-Ms
Exposure to a Face. Psychological Science, 17 (7), 592–598. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-
9280.2006.01750.x
Wilson, E. O. (1978). On human nature. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Wilson, E. O. (2012). The social conquest of earth. New York: Liveright Publishing
Wilson, G. T. (1982). How Useful is Meta-analysis in Evaluating the Effects of Different
Psychological Therapies? Behavioural Psychotherapy, 10, 221–231.
Wilson, G. T. (1996). Manual-based treatments: The clinical application of research findings.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34, 295–314.
Wilson, G. T., & Rachman, S. J. (1983). Meta-analysis and the evaluation of psychotherapy
outcome: Limitations and liabilities. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 51 (1), 54.
Wolpe, J. (1952a). Experimental neuroses as learned behavior. British Journal of
Psychology, 43, 243–268.
Wolpe, J. (1952b). Objective psychotherapy of the neuroses. South African Medical Journal,
26, 825–829.
Wolpe, J. (1954). Reciprocal inhibition as the main basis of psychotherapeutic effects.
American Medical Association Archives of Neurological Psychiatry, 72, 205–226.
Wolpe, J. (1958). Psychotherapy by reciprocal inhibition. Palo Alto, CA: Stanford University.
Woody, G. E., Luborsky, L., McLellan, A. T., Obrien, C. P., Beck, A. T., Blaine, J., … Hole, A.
(1983). Psychotherapy for opiate addicts—does it help? Archives of General Psychiatry, 40
(6), 639–645.
Worrell, M., & Longmore, R. J. (2008). Challenging Hofmann’s negative thoughts: A rebuttal.
Clinical Psychology Review, 28 (1), 71–74. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2007.03.004
Zuroff, D. C., & Blatt, S. J. (2006). The therapeutic relationship in brief treatment of
depression: Contributions to clinical improvement and enhanced adaptive capacities. Journal
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74, 130–140.
Zuroff, D. C., Kelly, A. C., Leybman, M. J., Blatt, S. J., & Wampold, B. E. (2010). Between-
therapist and within-therapist differences in the quality of the therapeutic relationship: Effects
on maladjustment and self-critical perfectionism. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 66, 681–697.
doi: 10.1002/jclp.20683

Leave a Reply