Legality of Cannabis by U.S. Jurisdiction

Autism
SpecialtyPsychiatry, psychology, neuropsychology, pedagogy, , developmental psychology Edit this on Wikidata
Frequency80%

Autism is a brain development disorder that shows symptoms before a child is three years old and has a steady course with no remission. Its characteristic signs are impairments in social interaction and communication, as well as restricted and repetitive behavior.[1] These characteristics distinguish autism from milder pervasive developmental disorders (PDD) or autism spectrum disorders (ASD),[2] and reflect Leo Kanner's first descriptions of autism, which emphasized "autistic aloneness" and "insistence on sameness".[3]

Heritability contributes about 90% of the risk of a child developing autism, but the genetics of autism are complex and it is unclear which genes are responsible. In rare cases, autism is strongly associated with agents that cause birth defects.[4] Many other causes have been proposed, such as exposure of children to vaccines; these proposals are controversial and the vaccine hypotheses have no convincing scientific evidence.[5]

Autism affects many parts of the brain, but how this occurs is poorly understood.[6] Parents usually notice warning signs in the first year or two of their child's life. Rapid evaluation by a specialist is important[7] as, although there is no cure, early intervention may aid attempts to help children gain self-care and social skills. However, only a very few of these interventions are supported by scientific studies.[8] With severe autism independent living is unlikely, but with milder autism there are some success stories as adults,[9] and an autistic culture has developed, with some seeking a cure and others believing that autism is simply another way of being.[10]

Most recent reviews estimate a prevalence of 1–2 per 1,000 people for autism and close to 6 per 1,000 for ASD, with ASD averaging a 4.3:1 male-to-female ratio. The number of diagnosed autism cases has increased dramatically since the 1980s. Although some of this increase is undoubtedly due to changes in diagnostic practice, it is not known whether prevalence has increased as well.[11]

Classification

File:Asperger kl2.jpg
Hans Asperger described a form of ASD in the 1940s that later became known as Asperger syndrome.

Autism is a developmental disorder of the human brain: that is, it always begins during infancy or childhood, its impairments are strongly related to maturation, it has a steady course without remissions or relapses, and it affects various systems in human brains. It is one of the five pervasive developmental disorders (PDD) or autism spectrum disorders (ASD), which are characterized by widespread abnormalities of social interactions and communication, and by severely restricted interests and highly repetitive behavior. Of the other four ASD forms, Asperger syndrome is closest to autism; Rett syndrome and childhood disintegrative disorder may have causes unrelated to autism and have some autistic-like symptoms; and pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified (PDD-NOS) is diagnosed when criteria are not met for a more-specific disorder.[12] The main difference between autism and Asperger syndrome is that a diagnosis of Asperger's requires there be no clinically significant delay in language development.[13] The terminology of autism can be bewildering: autism, Asperger's, and PDD-NOS are sometimes collectively called the autistic disorders,[14] whereas autism itself is often called autistic disorder, childhood autism, or infantile autism. This article uses autism to refer to the classic autistic disorder; other sources sometimes use autism to mean autistic disorders or even ASD. ASD in turn is a subset of the broader autism phenotype (BAP), which describes individuals who may not have ASD but do have some autistic-like traits such as less eye contact.[15]

Autism covers a wide range, from an individual with severe impairments who may be silent, mentally disabled, and locked into hand flapping and rocking, to a less impaired individual who may have an active but odd social approach, narrowly focused interests, and verbose, pedantic communication.[16] Sometimes it is informally and controversially divided into low-, medium- and high-functioning autism (LFA, MFA, and HFA) based on IQ thresholds[17] or on how well the individual can accomplish the activities of daily living. It can also be divided into syndromal and non-syndromal autism, where the former is associated with severe or profound mental retardation or with a congenital syndrome having physical symptoms, such as tuberous sclerosis.[18] Individuals with Asperger's tend to perform better cognitively than those with autism, and it is unclear to what extent Asperger's, HFA, and non-syndromal autism represent separate or overlapping conditions.[19]

Some studies have reported diagnoses of autism in some children due to a loss of language or social skills, as opposed to a failure to make progress. Several terms are used for this phenomenon, including regressive autism, setback autism, and developmental stagnation. The validity of this distinction remains controversial; it is possible regressive autism is a specific subtype.[20][21]

Characteristics

Autism is distinguished by a pattern of symptoms rather than one single symptom. The main characteristics are impairments in social interaction, impairments in communication, restricted interests and repetitive behavior. Other aspects, such as atypical eating, are also common but are not essential for diagnosis.

Social development

Autistic people have social impairments and often lack the intuition about others that many people take for granted. Noted autistic Temple Grandin described her inability to understand the social communication of neurotypicals as leaving her feeling "like an anthropologist on Mars".[22]

Social impairments become apparent early in childhood. Autistic infants show less attention to social stimuli, smile and look at others less often, and respond less to their own name. Autistic toddlers have more striking social deviance: for example, they have less eye contact and anticipatory postures and they are less able to use other people's bodies as tools.[21] Three- to five-year-old autistic children are less likely to exhibit social understanding, approach others spontaneously, imitate and respond to emotions, communicate nonverbally, and take turns with others. However, they do form attachments to their primary caregivers.[23] They display moderately less attachment security than usual, though this difference disappears in groups containing children with higher mental development or with less severe ASD.[24]

Contrary to common belief, autistic children do not prefer to be alone. Making and maintaining friendships often proves to be difficult for those with autism. For them the quality of friendships, not the number of friends, predicts how lonely they are.[25]

There are many anecdotal reports but few systematic studies of aggression and violence in individuals with ASD. The limited data suggest that in children with mental retardation, autism is associated with aggression, destruction of property, and tantrums. Dominick et al. interviewed parents of 67 children with ASD and reported that about two thirds of the children had periods of severe tantrums and about one third had a history of aggression, with tantrums significantly more common than in children with a history of language impairment.[26]

Communication

About a third to a half of autistic individuals do not develop enough natural speech to meet daily communication needs.[27] Communications differences may be present from the first year of life, and may include delayed onset of babbling, unusual gestures, diminished responsiveness, and desynchronization of vocal patterns with the caregiver. In the second and third years, autistic children have less frequent and less diverse babbling, consonants, words, and word combinations; their gestures are less-often integrated with words. Autistic children are less likely to make requests or share experiences and more likely to simply repeat others' words (echolalia)[20] or reverse pronouns.[3] Autistic children may have difficulty with imaginative play and with developing symbols into language.[20] They are more likely to have problems understanding pointing; for example, they may look at a pointing hand instead of the pointed-at object.[21]

In a pair of studies, high-functioning autistic children aged 8–15 performed equally well, and adults better, than individually matched controls at basic language tasks like vocabulary and spelling. Both autistic groups performed worse than controls at complex language tasks like figurative language, comprehension, and making inferences. As people are often sized up initially from their basic language skills, these studies suggest that people speaking to autistic individuals are more likely to overestimate what their audience comprehends.[28]

Repetitive behavior

A young boy with autism, and the precise line of toys he made

Autistic individuals display many forms of repetitive or restricted behavior, which the Repetitive Behavior Scale-Revised (RBS-R) categorizes as follows:

  • Stereotypy is apparently purposeless movement, such as hand flapping, head rolling, body rocking, or spinning a plate. Sometimes it is called self-stimulation or "stimming", though stereotypy and stimming are somewhat different notions.
  • Compulsive behavior is intended and appears to follow rules, such as arranging objects in a certain way.
  • Sameness is resistance to change, for example, insisting that the furniture not be moved, or refusing to be interrupted.
  • Ritualistic behavior performs daily activities the same way each time, such as an unvarying menu or dressing ritual.
  • Restricted behavior is limited in focus, interest, or activity, such as preoccupation with a single television program.
  • Self-injury includes movements that injure or can injure the person, such as biting oneself. Dominick et al. reported that self-injury at some point affected about 30% of children with ASD.[26]

No single repetitive behavior is identified with autism, but only autism appears to have an elevated pattern of occurrence and severity of these behaviors.[29]

Other symptoms

Autistic individuals may have symptoms that are independent of the diagnosis but can affect the individual or the family.[7] As many as 10% of individuals with ASD show unusual abilities ranging from splinter skills like trivia memorization to the extraordinarily rare talents of prodigious autistic savants.[30]

Unusual responses to sensory stimuli are more common and prominent in autistic children, though there is not good evidence that sensory symptoms differentiate autism from other developmental disorders.[31] The responses may be more common in children: a pair of studies found that autistic children had impaired tactile perception while autistic adults did not. The same two studies also found that autistic individuals had more problems with complex memory and reasoning tasks such as Twenty Questions; these problems were somewhat more marked among adults.[28] Several studies have reported associated motor problems that include poor muscle tone, poor motor planning, and toe walking; ASD is not associated with severe motor disturbances.[32]

Atypical eating occurs in about three fourths of children with ASD, so frequently that it used to be a diagnostic indicator. Selectivity is the most common problem, though eating rituals and food refusal also occur;[26] this does not appear to result in malnutrition. Some children with autism also have gastrointestinal (GI) symptoms, but there is a lack of published rigorous data to support the theory that autistic children have more or different GI symptoms than usual.[33]

Sleep problems are known to be more common in children with developmental disabilities, and there is some evidence that autistic children are more likely to have even more sleep problems than children with other developmental disabilities, with problems that include difficulty falling asleep, frequent nighttime awakenings, and early morning awakenings. Dominick et al. found that about two thirds of children with ASD had a history of sleep problems.[26]

Causes

Although many genetic and environmental causes have been proposed for autism, its theory of causation is still incomplete.[34] Tracking down the causes is often hard, and some researchers argue this is because autism is not a single disorder, but rather a triad of core aspects (social impairment, communication difficulties, and repetitive behaviors) that have distinct causes but often co-occur.[35]

Genetic factors are the most significant cause for autism spectrum disorders. Early studies of twins found the average concordance for identical twins to be 70% and estimated heritability to be over 90%, in other words, that genetics explains over 90% of whether a child will develop autism. Many of the non-autistic co-twins had learning or social disabilities. For adult siblings the risk for having one or more features of the broader autism phenotype might be as high as 30%.[36]

The genetics of autism is complex.[14] First, for each autistic individual, mutations in more than one gene may be implicated. Second, mutations in different sets of genes may be involved in different autistic individuals. Third, there may be significant interactions among mutations in several genes or between the environment and mutated genes. By identifying genetic markers inherited with autism in family studies, numerous candidate genes have been located, most of which encode proteins involved in neural development and function.[37] However, for most of the candidate genes, the actual mutations that increase the risk for autism have not been identified. Typically autism cannot be traced to a single-gene mutation or a single chromosome abnormality such as fragile X syndrome or 22q13 deletion syndrome.[18][38]

Deletion (1), duplication (2) and inversion (3) are all chromosome abnormalities that have been implicated in autism.[39]

The large number of autistic individuals with unaffected family members may result from copy number variations (CNVs)—spontaneous alterations in the genetic material during meiosis that delete or duplicate genetic material. Sporadic (non-inherited) cases have been examined to identify candidate genetic loci involved in autism. Using array comparative genomic hybridization (array CGH), a technique for detecting CNVs, one study found them in 10% of families with one affected child.[40] Some of the altered loci were identified in previous studies of inherited autism; many were unique to the sporadic cases examined in this study. Hence a substantial fraction of autism may be highly heritable but not inherited: that is, the mutation that causes the autism is new in the child and is not in the parent. The fraction of autism traceable to a genetic cause may grow to 30–40% as the resolution of array CGH improves.[39] The Autism Genome Project database contains genetic linkage and CNV data that connect autism to genetic loci and suggest that the genetic influence may extend to every human chromosome.[41]

Teratogens (agents that cause birth defects) related to autism risk include exposure of the embryo to thalidomide, valproic acid, or misoprostol, or to rubella infection in the mother. These cases are rare.[42] All known teratogens appear to act during the first eight weeks from conception, and though this does not exclude the possibility that autism can be initiated or affected later, it is strong evidence that autism arises very early in development.[4] Other possible contributors to autism include gastrointestinal or immune system abnormalities, allergies, and exposure of children to drugs, vaccines, infection, certain foods,[43] or heavy metals; the evidence for these risk factors is anecdotal and has not been confirmed by reliable studies,[5] and extensive further searches are underway.[42] Parents may first become aware of autistic symptoms in their child around the time of a routine vaccination. The overwhelming majority of scientific evidence shows no causal connection between the measles-mumps-rubella vaccine and autism, or between the vaccine preservative thiomersal and autism, but parental concern has led to decreasing uptake of childhood immunizations and increasing likelihood of measles outbreaks.[44]

Mechanism

Despite extensive investigation, how autism occurs is not well understood. Its mechanism can be divided into two areas: the pathophysiology of brain structures and processes associated with autism, and the neuropsychological linkages between brain structures and behaviors.[6]

Pathophysiology

Autism affects many parts of the brain.

Autism appears to result from developmental factors that affect many or all functional brain systems, as opposed to localized physical damage.[38] Many major structures of the human brain have been implicated. Consistent neuroanatomical abnormalities have been found in the development of the cerebral cortex; and in the cerebellum and related inferior olive, which have a significant decrease in the number of Purkinje cells. Brain weight and volume and head circumference tend to be greater in autistic children; the effects of these are unknown.[45] It may be due to poorly regulated growth of neurons.[6]

Interactions between the immune system and the nervous system begin early during embryogenesis, and successful neurodevelopment depends on a balanced immune response. Several symptoms consistent with a poorly regulated immune response have been reported in autistic children. It is possible that aberrant immune activity during critical periods of neurodevelopment is part of the mechanism of some forms of ASD.[46] Given the lack of data in this area, it is still hard to draw conclusions about the role of immune factors in autism.[47]

Several neurotransmitter abnormalities have been detected in autism, notably increased blood levels of serotonin. It is unclear whether these lead to structural or behavioral abnormalities.[6]

The mirror neuron system (MNS) theory of autism hypothesizes that distortion in the development of the MNS interferes with imitation and leads to autism's core features of social impairment and communication difficulties. The MNS operates when an animal performs an action or observes another animal of the same species perform the same action. The MNS may contribute to an individual's understanding of other people by enabling the modeling of their behavior via embodied simulation of their actions, intentions, and emotions.[48] Several studies have tested this hypothesis by demonstrating structural abnormalities in MNS regions of individuals with ASD, delay in the activation in the core circuit for imitation in individuals with Asperger's, and a correlation between reduced MNS activity and severity of the syndrome in children with ASD.[49]

Functional magnetic resonance imaging provides some evidence for the underconnectivity theory of autism.

The underconnectivity theory of autism hypothesizes that autism is marked by underfunctioning high-level neural connections and synchronization, along with an excess of low-level processes.[50] Evidence for this theory has been found in functional neuroimaging studies on autistic individuals[28] and by a brain wave study that suggested that adults with ASD have local overconnectivity in the cortex and weak functional connections between the frontal lobe and the rest of the cortex.[51] Other evidence suggests the underconnectivity is mainly within each hemisphere of the cortex and that autism is a disorder of the association cortex.[52]

Neuropsychology

Two major categories of cognitive theories have been proposed about the links between autistic brains and behavior.

The first category focuses on deficits in social cognition. Hyper-systemizing hypothesizes that autistic individuals can systematize—that is, they can develop internal rules of operation to handle internal events—but are less effective at empathizing by handling events generated by other agents.[17] It extends the extreme male brain theory, which hypothesizes that autism is an extreme case of the male brain, defined psychometrically as individuals in whom systemizing is better than empathizing.[53] This in turn is related to the earlier theory of mind, which hypothesizes that autistic behavior arises because autistic people cannot easily think about thinking,[54] and which is mapped well from the mirror neuron system theory of autism.[49]

The second category focuses on nonsocial or general processing. Executive dysfunction hypothesizes that autistic behavior results in part from deficits in flexible planning, working memory, and impulse control. One strength of the theory is predicting stereotyped behavior and narrow interests.[55] Weak central coherence theory hypothesizes that a limited ability to see the big picture underlies the central disturbance in autism. One strength of this theory is predicting special talents and peaks in performance in autistic people.[56] A related theory—enhanced perceptual functioning—focuses more on the superiority of low-level perceptual operations in autistic individuals.[57] The latter two theories map well from the underconnectivity theory of autism.

Neither category is satisfactory on its own. Social cognition theories poorly address autism's rigid and repetitive behaviors, while the nonsocial theories have difficulty explaining social impairment and communication difficulties.[35]

Screening

Parents are usually the first to notice unusual behaviors in their child: for example, a baby might seem unresponsive to people or might focus intently on one item for long periods of time. Children who seem to have been developing normally might also show signs of autism, for example by becoming silent or indifferent to social overtures.[58] As delay in evaluation may postpone treatment and affect long-term outcome, any of the following warning signs is reason to have a child evaluated by a specialist immediately:

  • No babbling by 12 months.
  • No gesturing (pointing, waving goodbye, etc.) by 12 months.
  • No single words by 16 months.
  • No two-word spontaneous phrases (not including echolalia) by 24 months.
  • Any loss of any language or social skills, at any age.[7]

The American Academy of Pediatrics recommends that all children be screened for ASD at the 18-month and the 24- or 30-month well-child doctor visits, using autism-specific developmental screening tools.[59] In contrast, the UK National Screening Committee recommends against screening for ASD in the general population, because screening tools have not been fully validated and interventions lack sufficient evidence for effectiveness.[60]

Genetic screening for autism is generally still impractical. As genetic tests are developed several ethical, legal, and social issues will emerge. Commercial availability of tests may precede adequate understanding of how to use test results, given the complexity of autism's genetics.[61]

Diagnosis

Autism is defined in section 299.00 of DSM-IV-TR as exhibiting at least six symptoms total, including at least two symptoms of qualitative impairment in social interaction, at least one symptom of qualitative impairment in communication, and at least one symptom of restricted and repetitive behavior. Sample symptoms include lack of social or emotional reciprocity, stereotyped and repetitive use of language or idiosyncratic language, and persistent preoccupation with parts of objects. Onset must be prior to age three years, with delays or abnormal functioning in either social interaction, language as used in social communication, or symbolic or imaginative play. The disturbance must not be better accounted for by Rett syndrome or childhood disintegrative disorder.[1] ICD-10 section F84.0 uses essentially the same definition.[2]

Several diagnostic instruments are available. Two are commonly used in autism research: the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R) is a semistructured parent interview, and the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) uses observation and interaction with the child. The Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS) is used widely in clinical environments to assess severity of autism based on observation of children.[21]

A pediatrician commonly performs a preliminary investigation by taking developmental history and physically examining the child. If warranted, diagnosis and evaluations are conducted with help from ASD specialists, observing and assessing cognitive, communication, family, and other factors using standardized tools, and taking into account any associated medical conditions. A differential diagnosis at this stage might consider several other possibilities, including ADHD, anxiety, depression, hearing impairment, mental retardation, obsessive-compulsive disorder, and specific speech disorders. Since early diagnosis is important, in the UK the National Autism Plan for Children recommends at most 30 weeks from first concern to completed diagnosis and assessment, though few cases are handled that quickly in practice.[62] ASD can sometimes be diagnosed by age 14 months,[63] but a 2006 US study found the average age of first evaluation by a qualified professional was 48 months and of formal ASD diagnosis was 61 months, reflecting an average 13-month delay, all far above recommendations.[64]

Underdiagnosis and overdiagnosis are problems in marginal cases, and much of the recent increase in the number of reported ASD cases is likely due to changes in diagnostic practices. The increasing popularity of drug treatment options and the expansion of benefits has given providers incentives to diagnose ASD, resulting in some overdiagnosis of children with uncertain symptoms. Conversely, the cost of screening and diagnosis and the challenge of obtaining payment can inhibit or delay diagnosis.[65] It is particularly hard to diagnose autism among the visually impaired, partly because some of its diagnostic criteria depend on vision, and partly because autistic symptoms overlap with those of common blindness syndromes.[66]

The symptoms of autism and ASD begin early in childhood but are occasionally missed. Adults may seek retrospective diagnoses to help them or their friends and family understand themselves, to help their employers make adjustments, or in some locations to claim disability living allowances or other benefits.[67]

Treatment

Early intervention is important. No single treatment is best and treatment is typically tailored to the child's needs. Intensive, sustained special education programs and behavior therapy can help children acquire self-care, social, and job skills. Among the available approaches, applied behavior analysis (ABA) has demonstrated therapeutic effect in clinical trials that is at least as good as other treatments in promoting social and language development and in reducing behaviors that interfere with functioning and learning; ABA focuses on teaching tasks one-on-one using the behaviorist principles of stimulus, response and reward. Cognitive therapies based on comprehensive center-based programs are a common alternative: for example, TEACCH focuses more on structuring the physical environment and using visual supports to ease language development tasks.[68] A 2005 California study found that early intensive behavior analytic treatment, a form of ABA, was substantially more effective for preschool children with autism than the mixture of methods provided in many programs.[69] The limited research on the effectiveness of adult residential programs shows mixed results.[70]

Medications are often used to treat problems associated with ASD. More than half of U.S. children diagnosed with ASD are prescribed psychoactive drugs or anticonvulsants, with the most common drug classes being antidepressants, stimulants, and antipsychotics.[71] The FDA has approved the antipsychotic risperidone for treating symptomatic irritability in autistic children and adolescents.[72] Several other drugs are prescribed off-label, which means they have not been been approved for treating ASD. For example, serotonin reuptake inhibitors and dopamine blockers can sometimes reduce some symptoms.[6] However, someone with ASD may respond atypically to medications, the medications can have adverse side effects, and no known medication relieves autism's core symptoms of social and communication impairments.[58]

Many other therapies and interventions are available. Few are supported by scientific studies.[8][73][74] Treatment approaches lack empirical support in quality-of-life contexts, and many programs focus on success measures that lack predictive validity and real-world relevance.[25] Scientific evidence appears to matter less to service providers than program marketing, training availability, and parent requests.[75] Even if they do not help, conservative treatments such as changes in diet are probably harmless aside from their bother and cost.[43] Dubious invasive treatments are a much more serious matter: for example, in 2005, botched chelation therapy killed a 5-year-old autistic boy.[44]

Treatment is expensive; indirect costs are more so. A U.S. study estimated the average additional lifetime cost due exclusively to autism to be $3.2 million in 2003 US dollars for an autistic individual born in 2000, with about 10% medical care, 30% nonmedical care such as child care and education, and 60% the lost economic productivity of individuals and their parents.[76] A British study estimated an average lifetime cost of ₤2.4 million in 1997–1998 British pounds.[77] Legal rights to treatment are complex, vary with location and age, and require advocacy by caregivers.[74] Publicly-supported programs are often inadequate or inappropriate for a given child, and unreimbursed out-of-pocket medical or therapy expenses are associated with likelihood of family financial problems.[78] After childhood, key treatment issues include residential care, job training and placement, sexuality, social skills, and estate planning.[74]

Prognosis

No cure is known for autism. Most children with autism lack social support, meaningful relationships, future employment opportunities or self-determination.[25] Although core difficulties remain, the severity of symptoms often becomes less marked in later childhood.[79] Few high-quality studies address long-term prognosis. Some adults show modest improvement in some symptoms, but some decline; no study has focused on autism after midlife.[80] Acquiring language before age 6, having IQ above 50, and having a marketable skill all predict better outcomes; independent living is unlikely with severe autism.[81] A 2004 British study of 68 adults who were diagnosed before 1980 as autistic children with IQ above 50 found that 12% achieved a high level of independence as adults, 10% had some friends and were generally in work but required some support, 19% had some independence but were generally living at home and needed considerable support and supervision in daily living, 46% needed specialist residential provision from facilities specializing in ASD with a high level of support and very limited autonomy, and 12% needed high-level hospital care.[9] Changes in diagnostic practice and increased availability of effective early intervention make it unclear whether these findings can be generalized to recently diagnosed children.[11]

Epidemiology

Estimates of the prevalence of autism vary widely depending on diagnostic criteria, age of children screened, and geographical location.[82] One review estimated a prevalence of at least 1.3 per 1,000 for autism and 6.0–6.5 per 1,000 for ASD; PDD-NOS was the vast majority of ASD, Asperger's was about 0.3 per 1,000 and the atypical forms childhood disintegrative disorder and Rett syndrome were much rarer.[83] Most recent reviews tend to estimate a prevalence of 1–2 per 1,000 for autism and close to 6 per 1,000 for ASD.[11] A 2006 study of nearly 57,000 British nine- and ten-year-olds reported a prevalence of 3.89 per 1,000 for autism and 11.61 per 1,000 for ASD; these higher figures could be associated with broadening diagnostic criteria.[84]

The risk of autism is associated with several prenatal and perinatal risk factors. A 2007 review of risk factors found associated parental characteristics that included advanced maternal age, advanced paternal age, and maternal place of birth outside Europe or North America, and also found associated obstetric conditions that included low birth weight and gestation duration, and hypoxia during childbirth.[85]

About 10–15% of autism cases are caused by a known medical condition such as Prader-Willi syndrome or tuberous sclerosis[14] and ASD is associated with several other genetic disorders.[86] Autism is associated with mental retardation: a 2001 British study of 26 autistic children found about 30% with intelligence in the normal range (IQ above 70), 50% with mild to moderate retardation, and about 20% with severe to profound retardation (IQ below 35). For ASD other than autism the association is much weaker: the same study reported about 94% of 65 children with PDD-NOS or Asperger's had normal intelligence.[87] ASD is also associated with epilepsy, with variations in risk due to age, cognitive level, and type of language disorder.[88] Boys are at higher risk for autism than girls. The ASD sex ratio averages 4.3:1 and is greatly modified by cognitive impairment: it may be close to 2:1 with mental retardation and more than 5.5:1 without. Recent studies have found no association with socioeconomic status, and have reported inconsistent results about associations with race or ethnicity.[11] Phobias, depression and other psychopathological disorders have often been described along with ASD but this has not been assessed systematically.[89]

Autism's incidence is less useful than its prevalence, as the disorder starts long before it is diagnosed, and the gap between initiation and diagnosis is influenced by many factors unrelated to risk. Instead, attention is focused mostly on whether prevalence is increasing with time. Earlier prevalence estimates were lower, centering at about 0.5 per 1,000 for autism during the 1960s and 1970s and about 1 per 1,000 in the 1980s, as opposed to today's 1–2 per 1,000.[11]

Reports of autism cases grew dramatically in the U.S. in 1996–2005. It is not known how much, if any, growth came from real changes in autism's prevalence.

The number of reported cases of autism increased dramatically in the 1990s and early 2000s. This increase is largely attributable to changes in diagnostic practices, referral patterns, availability of services, age at diagnosis, and public awareness.[90] though as-yet-unidentified contributing environmental risk factors cannot be ruled out.[5] A widely-cited 2002 pilot study concluded that some of the observed increase in autism in California was real,[91] but a 2006 analysis found that special education data poorly measured prevalence because so many cases were undiagnosed, and that the 1994–2003 U.S. increase was associated with declines in other diagnostic categories, indicating that diagnostic substitution had occurred.[92]

It is not known whether any of the increase was real. A real increase would suggest more attention and funding on changing environmental factors, as opposed to continuing to focus on genetics. It is conceivable that some environmental factors may affect people with a specific genetic constitution.[42] Chris Johnson, co-chair of the American Academy of Pediatrics Autism Expert Panel, summed up the 2005 state of the issue by saying, "There is a chance we're seeing a true rise, but right now I don't think anybody can answer that question for sure."[93]

History

A few examples of autism and its treatment can be found from long before it was named. The Table Talk of Martin Luther contains a story of a 12-year-old boy who may have been severely autistic.[94] According to Luther's notetaker Mathesius, Luther thought the boy was a soulless mass of flesh possessed by the devil, and suggested that he be suffocated.[95] Victor of Aveyron, a feral child caught in 1798, showed several signs of autism; the medical student Jean Itard treated him with a behavioral program designed to help him form social attachments and to induce speech via imitation.[96]

The New Latin word autismus (English translation autism) was coined by the Swiss psychiatrist Eugen Bleuler in 1910 as he was defining symptoms of schizophrenia. He derived it from the Greek word autos (αὐτός, meaning self), and used it to mean morbid self-admiration, referring to "autistic withdrawal of the patient to his fantasies, against which any influence from outside becomes an intolerable disturbance."[97]

File:Kanner kl2.jpg
Leo Kanner introduced the label early infantile autism in 1943.

The word autism took its modern sense in 1943 when Leo Kanner of the Johns Hopkins Hospital reported 11 children with striking behavioral similarities and introduced the label early infantile autism.[3] He suggested autism to describe the children's lack of interest in other people. Almost every characteristic described in Kanner's first paper on the subject is still regarded as typical of the autistic spectrum of disorders.[35] About the same time, the Viennese pediatrician Hans Asperger described a similar form of ASD now known as Asperger syndrome, though for various reasons it was not widely recognized as a separate syndrome until 1981.[96]

Kanner's reuse of autism led to decades of confused terminology like "infantile schizophrenia", and child psychiatry's focus on maternal deprivation during the mid-1900s led to misconceptions of autism as an infant's response to "refrigerator mothers". Starting in the late 1960s autism was established as a separate syndrome by demonstrating that it is lifelong, distinguishing it from mental retardation and schizophrenia and from other developmental disorders, and demonstrating the benefits of involving parents in active programs of therapy.[98] Since then, the rise of parent organizations and the destigmatization of childhood ASD have deeply affected how we view ASD, its boundaries, and its treatments.[96] The Internet has helped autistic individuals bypass nonverbal cues and emotional sharing that they find so hard to deal with, and has given them a way to form online communities and work remotely.[99] An autistic culture has developed: some in the community seek a cure, while others believe that autism is simply another way of being.[10]

Notes

  1. ^ a b American Psychiatric Association (2000). "Diagnostic criteria for 299.00 Autistic Disorder". Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed., text revision (DSM-IV-TR) ed.). ISBN 0890420254. {{cite book}}: |access-date= requires |url= (help); External link in |chapterurl= (help); Unknown parameter |chapterurl= ignored (|chapter-url= suggested) (help)
  2. ^ a b World Health Organization (2006). "F84. Pervasive developmental disorders". International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems (10th ed. (ICD-10) ed.). {{cite book}}: |access-date= requires |url= (help); External link in |chapterurl= (help); Unknown parameter |chapterurl= ignored (|chapter-url= suggested) (help)
  3. ^ a b c Kanner L (1943). "Autistic disturbances of affective contact". Nerv Child. 2: 217–50. Retrieved 2007-06-25.
  4. ^ a b Arndt TL, Stodgell CJ, Rodier PM (2005). "The teratology of autism". Int J Dev Neurosci. 23 (2–3): 189–99. doi:10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2004.11.001. PMID 15749245.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  5. ^ a b c Rutter M (2005). "Incidence of autism spectrum disorders: changes over time and their meaning". Acta Paediatr. 94 (1): 2–15. PMID 15858952.
  6. ^ a b c d e Penn HE (2006). "Neurobiological correlates of autism: a review of recent research". Child Neuropsychol. 12 (1): 57–79. doi:10.1080/09297040500253546. PMID 16484102.
  7. ^ a b c Filipek PA, Accardo PJ, Baranek GT; et al. (1999). "The screening and diagnosis of autistic spectrum disorders". J Autism Dev Disord. 29 (6): 439–84. doi:10.1023/A:1021943802493. {{cite journal}}: Explicit use of et al. in: |author= (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) Erratum (2000). J Autism Dev Disord 30 (1): 81. doi:10.1023/A:1017256313409. PMID 10638459. This paper represents a consensus of representatives from nine professional and four parent organizations in the U.S.
  8. ^ a b Francis K (2005). "Autism interventions: a critical update" (PDF). Dev Med Child Neurol. 47 (7): 493–9. PMID 15991872. Retrieved 2007-07-18.
  9. ^ a b Howlin P, Goode S, Hutton J, Rutter M (2004). "Adult outcome for children with autism". J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 45 (2): 212–29. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00215.x. PMID 14982237.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  10. ^ a b Gal L (2007-06-28). "Who says autism's a disease?". Haaretz. Retrieved 2007-07-16. {{cite news}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  11. ^ a b c d e Newschaffer CJ, Croen LA, Daniels J; et al. (2007). "The epidemiology of autism spectrum disorders". Annu Rev Public Health. 28: 235–58. doi:10.1146/annurev.publhealth.28.021406.144007. PMID 17367287. {{cite journal}}: Explicit use of et al. in: |author= (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  12. ^ Lord C, Cook EH, Leventhal BL, Amaral DG (2000). "Autism spectrum disorders" (PDF). Neuron. 28 (2): 355–63. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(00)00115-X. PMID 11144346. Retrieved 2007-07-18.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  13. ^ American Psychiatric Association (2000). "Diagnostic criteria for 299.80 Asperger's Disorder (AD)". Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed., text revision (DSM-IV-TR) ed.). ISBN 0890420254. {{cite book}}: |access-date= requires |url= (help); External link in |chapterurl= (help); Unknown parameter |chapterurl= ignored (|chapter-url= suggested) (help)
  14. ^ a b c Freitag CM (2007). "The genetics of autistic disorders and its clinical relevance: a review of the literature". Mol Psychiatry. 12 (1): 2–22. doi:10.1038/sj.mp.4001896. PMID 17033636. Retrieved 2007-07-18.
  15. ^ Piven J, Palmer P, Jacobi D, Childress D, Arndt S (1997). "Broader autism phenotype: evidence from a family history study of multiple-incidence autism families" (PDF). Am J Psychiatry. 154 (2): 185–90. PMID 9016266. Retrieved 2007-07-18.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  16. ^ Happé F (1999). "Understanding assets and deficits in autism: why success is more interesting than failure" (PDF). Psychologist. 12 (11): 540–547. Retrieved 2007-07-15.
  17. ^ a b Baron-Cohen S (2006). "The hyper-systemizing, assortative mating theory of autism". Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 30 (5): 865–72. doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2006.01.010. PMID 16519981.
  18. ^ a b Cohen D, Pichard N, Tordjman S; et al. (2005). "Specific genetic disorders and autism: clinical contribution towards their identification". J Autism Dev Disord. 35 (1): 103–16. doi:10.1007/s10803-004-1038-2. PMID 15796126. {{cite journal}}: Explicit use of et al. in: |author= (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  19. ^ Schopler E, Mesibov GB, Kunce LJ (eds) (1998). Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism?. Springer. ISBN 0306457466. {{cite book}}: |author= has generic name (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  20. ^ a b c Landa R (2007). "Early communication development and intervention for children with autism". Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. 13 (1): 16–25. doi:10.1002/mrdd.20134. PMID 17326115.
  21. ^ a b c d Volkmar F, Chawarska K, Klin A (2005). "Autism in infancy and early childhood". Annu Rev Psychol. 56: 315–36. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070159. PMID 15709938.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  22. ^ Sacks O (1995). An Anthropologist on Mars: Seven Paradoxical Tales. Knopf. ISBN 0679437851.
  23. ^ Sigman M, Dijamco A, Gratier M, Rozga A (2004). "Early detection of core deficits in autism". Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. 10 (4): 221–33. doi:10.1002/mrdd.20046. PMID 15666338.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  24. ^ Rutgers AH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van IJzendoorn MH, van Berckelaer-Onnes IA (2004). "Autism and attachment: a meta-analytic review". J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 45 (6): 1123–34. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.t01-1-00305.x. PMID 15257669.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  25. ^ a b c Burgess AF, Gutstein SE (2007). "Quality of life for people with autism: raising the standard for evaluating successful outcomes". Child and Adolescent Mental Health. 12 (2): 80–86. doi:10.1111/j.1475-3588.2006.00432.x.
  26. ^ a b c d Dominick KC, Davis NO, Lainhart J, Tager-Flusberg H, Folstein S (2007). "Atypical behaviors in children with autism and children with a history of language impairment". Res Dev Disabil. 28 (2): 145–62. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2006.02.003. PMID 16581226.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  27. ^ Noens I, van Berckelaer-Onnes I, Verpoorten R, van Duijn G (2006). "The ComFor: an instrument for the indication of augmentative communication in people with autism and intellectual disability". J Intellect Disabil Res. 50 (9): 621–32. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2788.2006.00807.x. PMID 16901289.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  28. ^ a b c Williams DL, Goldstein G, Minshew NJ (2006). "Neuropsychologic functioning in children with autism: further evidence for disordered complex information-processing". Child neuropsychol. 12 (4–5): 279–98. doi:10.1080/09297040600681190. PMID 16911973. Retrieved 2007-06-30.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  29. ^ Bodfish JW, Symons FJ, Parker DE, Lewis MH (2000). "Varieties of repetitive behavior in autism: comparisons to mental retardation". J Autism Dev Disord. 30 (3): 237–43. doi:10.1023/A:1005596502855. PMID 11055459.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  30. ^ Treffert DA (2007). "Savant syndrome: an extraordinary condition". Wisconsin Medical Society. Retrieved 2007-07-13.
  31. ^ Rogers SJ, Ozonoff S (2005). "Annotation: what do we know about sensory dysfunction in autism? A critical review of the empirical evidence". J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 46 (12): 1255–68. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01431.x. PMID 16313426.
  32. ^ Ming X, Brimacombe M, Wagner GC (2007). "Prevalence of motor impairment in autism spectrum disorders". Brain Dev. doi:10.1016/j.braindev.2007.03.002. PMID 17467940.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  33. ^ Erickson CA, Stigler KA, Corkins MR, Posey DJ, Fitzgerald JF, McDougle CJ (2005). "Gastrointestinal factors in autistic disorder: a critical review". J Autism Dev Disord. 35 (6): 713–27. doi:10.1007/s10803-005-0019-4. PMID 16267642.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  34. ^ Trottier G, Srivastava L, Walker CD (1999). "Etiology of infantile autism: a review of recent advances in genetic and neurobiological research". J Psychiatry Neurosci. 24 (2): 103–115. PMID 10212552. Retrieved 2007-07-16.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  35. ^ a b c Happé F, Ronald A, Plomin R (2006). "Time to give up on a single explanation for autism". Nat Neurosci. 9 (10): 1218–20. doi:10.1038/nn1770. PMID 17001340.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  36. ^ Folstein SE, Rosen-Sheidley B (2001). "Genetics of autism: complex aetiology for a heterogeneous disorder". Nat Rev Genet. 2 (12): 943–55. doi:10.1038/35103559. PMID 11733747.
  37. ^ Persico AM, Bourgeron T (2006). "Searching for ways out of the autism maze: genetic, epigenetic and environmental clues". Trends Neurosci. 29 (7): 349–58. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2006.05.010. PMID 16808981.
  38. ^ a b Müller RA (2007). "The study of autism as a distributed disorder". Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. 13 (1): 85–95. doi:10.1002/mrdd.20141. PMID 17326118.
  39. ^ a b Beaudet AL (2007). "Autism: highly heritable but not inherited". Nat Med. 13 (5): 534–6. doi:10.1038/nm0507-534. PMID 17479094.
  40. ^ Sebat J, Lakshmi B, Malhotra D; et al. (2007). "Strong association of de novo copy number mutations with autism". Science. 316 (5823): 445–9. doi:10.1126/science.1138659. PMID 17363630. {{cite journal}}: Explicit use of et al. in: |author= (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  41. ^ Autism Genome Project Consortium (2007). "Mapping autism risk loci using genetic linkage and chromosomal rearrangements". Nat Genet. 39 (3): 319–28. doi:10.1038/ng1985. PMID 17322880.
  42. ^ a b c Szpir M (2006). "Tracing the origins of autism: a spectrum of new studies". Environ Health Perspect. 114 (7): A412–8. PMID 16835042. Retrieved 2007-07-09.
  43. ^ a b Christison GW, Ivany K (2006). "Elimination diets in autism spectrum disorders: any wheat amidst the chaff?". J Dev Behav Pediatr. 27 (2 Suppl 2): S162–71. PMID 16685183.
  44. ^ a b Doja A, Roberts W (2006). "Immunizations and autism: a review of the literature". Can J Neurol Sci. 33 (4): 341–6. PMID 17168158.
  45. ^ DiCicco-Bloom E, Lord C, Zwaigenbaum L; et al. (2006). "The developmental neurobiology of autism spectrum disorder". J Neurosci. 26 (26): 6897–906. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1712-06.2006. PMID 16807320. Retrieved 2007-07-17. {{cite journal}}: Explicit use of et al. in: |author= (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  46. ^ Ashwood P, Wills S, Van de Water J (2006). "The immune response in autism: a new frontier for autism research". J Leukoc Biol. 80 (1): 1–15. doi:10.1189/jlb.1205707. PMID 16698940. Retrieved 2007-07-18.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  47. ^ Krause I, He XS, Gershwin ME, Shoenfeld Y (2002). "Brief report: immune factors in autism: a critical review". J Autism Dev Disord. 32 (4): 337–45. doi:10.1023/A:1016391121003. PMID 12199139.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  48. ^ Williams JHG, Whiten A, Suddendorf T, Perrett DI (2001). "Imitation, mirror neurons and autism". Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 25 (4): 287–95. doi:10.1016/S0149-7634(01)00014-8. PMID 11445135.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  49. ^ a b Iacoboni M, Dapretto M (2006). "The mirror neuron system and the consequences of its dysfunction". Nat Rev Neurosci. 7 (12): 942–51. doi:10.1038/nrn2024. PMID 17115076.
  50. ^ Just MA, Cherkassky VL, Keller TA, Minshew NJ (2004). "Cortical activation and synchronization during sentence comprehension in high-functioning autism: evidence of underconnectivity". Brain. 127 (8): 1811–21. doi:10.1093/brain/awh199. PMID 15215213. Retrieved 2007-07-18.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  51. ^ Murias M, Webb SJ, Greenson J, Dawson G (2007). "Resting state cortical connectivity reflected in EEG coherence in individuals with autism". Biol Psychiatry. 62 (3): 270–3. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.11.012. PMID 17336944.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  52. ^ Minshew NJ, Williams DL (2007). "The new neurobiology of autism: cortex, connectivity, and neuronal organization". Arch Neurol. 64 (7): 945–950. PMID 17620483.
  53. ^ Baron-Cohen S (2002). "The extreme male brain theory of autism". Trends Cogn Sci. 6 (6): 248–254. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01904-6. PMID 12039606.
  54. ^ Baron-Cohen S, Leslie AM, Frith U (1985). "Does the autistic child have a 'theory of mind'?" (PDF). Cognition. 21 (1): 37–46. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8. PMID 2934210. Retrieved 2007-06-28.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  55. ^ Hill EL (2004). "Executive dysfunction in autism". Trends Cogn Sci. 8 (1): 26–32. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2004.01.001. PMID 14697400.
  56. ^ Happé F, Frith U (2006). "The weak coherence account: detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders". J Autism Dev Disord. 36 (1): 5–25. doi:10.1007/s10803-005-0039-0. PMID 16450045.
  57. ^ Mottron L, Dawson M, Soulières I, Hubert B, Burack J (2006). "Enhanced perceptual functioning in autism: an update, and eight principles of autistic perception". J Autism Dev Disord. 36 (1): 27–43. doi:10.1007/s10803-005-0040-7. PMID 16453071.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  58. ^ a b Strock, M (2007). "Autism spectrum disorders (pervasive developmental disorders)". NIH-04-5511. National Institute of Mental Health. Retrieved 2007-06-27. {{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help)
  59. ^ Council on Children With Disabilities, Section on Developmental Behavioral Pediatrics, Bright Futures Steering Committee, Medical Home Initiatives for Children With Special Needs Project Advisory Committee (2006). "Identifying infants and young children with developmental disorders in the medical home: an algorithm for developmental surveillance and screening". Pediatrics. 118 (1): 405–20. doi:10.1542/peds.2006-1231.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) [Same authors] (2006). "Errata". Pediatrics. 118 (4): 1808–9. doi:10.1542/peds.2006-2405. PMID 16818591. Retrieved 2007-07-18.
  60. ^ Williams J, Brayne C (2006). "Screening for autism spectrum disorders: what is the evidence?". Autism. 10 (1): 11–35. doi:10.1177/1362361306057876. PMID 16522708.
  61. ^ McMahon WM, Baty BJ, Botkin J (2006). "Genetic counseling and ethical issues for autism". Am J Med Genet C Semin Med Genet. 142C (1): 52–7. doi:10.1002/ajmg.c.30082. PMID 16419100.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  62. ^ Dover CJ, Le Couteur A (2007). "How to diagnose autism". Arch Dis Child. 92 (6): 540–5. doi:10.1136/adc.2005.086280. PMID 17515625.
  63. ^ Landa RJ, Holman KC, Garrett-Mayer E (2007). "Social and communication development in toddlers with early and later diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders". Arch Gen Psychiatry. 64 (7): 853–64. PMID 17606819.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  64. ^ Wiggins LD, Baio J, Rice C (2006). "Examination of the time between first evaluation and first autism spectrum diagnosis in a population-based sample". J Dev Behav Pediatr. 27 (2 Suppl): S79–87. PMID 16685189.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  65. ^ Shattuck PT, Grosse SD (2007). "Issues related to the diagnosis and treatment of autism spectrum disorders". Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. 13 (2): 129–35. doi:10.1002/mrdd.20143. PMID 17563895.
  66. ^ Cass H (1998). "Visual impairment and autism: current questions and future research". Autism. 2 (2): 117–38. doi:10.1177/1362361398022002.
  67. ^ "Why get a diagnosis as an adult?". The National Autistic Society. 2005. Retrieved 2007-06-29.
  68. ^ U.S. Dept. of Health and Human Services (1999). "Autism". Mental Health: A Report of the Surgeon General. Rockville, MD: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration, Center for Mental Health Services, National Institutes of Health, National Institute of Mental Health. {{cite book}}: External link in |chapterurl= (help); Unknown parameter |chapterurl= ignored (|chapter-url= suggested) (help) Retrieved on 2007-07-11.
  69. ^ Howard JS, Sparkman CR, Cohen HG, Green G, Stanislaw H (2005). "A comparison of intensive behavior analytic and eclectic treatments for young children with autism". Res Dev Disabil. 26 (4): 359–83. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2004.09.005. PMID 15766629.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  70. ^ Van Bourgondien ME, Reichle NC, Schopler E (2003). "Effects of a model treatment approach on adults with autism". J Autism Dev Disord. 33 (2): 131–40. doi:10.1023/A:1022931224934. PMID 12757352.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  71. ^ Oswald DP, Sonenklar NA (2007). "Medication use among children with autism spectrum disorders". J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 17 (3): 348–55. doi:10.1089/cap.2006.17303. PMID 17630868.
  72. ^ "FDA approves the first drug to treat irritability associated with autism, Risperdal" (Press release). FDA. 2006-10-06. Retrieved 2006-10-02. {{cite press release}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  73. ^ Lack of support for interventions:
  74. ^ a b c Aman MG (2005). "Treatment planning for patients with autism spectrum disorders". J Clin Psychiatry. 66 (Suppl 10): 38–45. PMID 16401149.
  75. ^ Stahmer AC, Collings NM, Palinkas LA (2005). "Early intervention practices for children with autism: descriptions from community providers". Focus Autism Other Dev Disabl. 20 (2): 66–79. PMID 16467905. Retrieved 2007-07-12.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  76. ^ Ganz ML (2007). "The lifetime distribution of the incremental societal costs of autism". Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 161 (4): 343–9. PMID 17404130. Retrieved 2007-07-18.
  77. ^ Järbrink K, Knapp M (2001). "The economic impact of autism in Britain". Autism. 5 (1): 7–22. doi:10.1177/1362361301005001002. PMID 11708392.
  78. ^ Sharpe DL, Baker DL (2007). "Financial issues associated with having a child with autism". J Fam Econ Iss. 28 (2): 247–64. doi:10.1007/s10834-007-9059-6.
  79. ^ Howlin P (2006). "Autism spectrum disorders". Psychiatry. 5 (9): 320–324. doi:10.1053/j.mppsy.2006.06.007.
  80. ^ Seltzer MM, Shattuck P, Abbeduto L, Greenberg JS (2004). "Trajectory of development in adolescents and adults with autism". Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. 10 (4): 234–47. doi:10.1002/mrdd.20038. PMID 15666341.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  81. ^ Tidmarsh L, Volkmar FR (2003). "Diagnosis and epidemiology of autism spectrum disorders". Can J Psychiatry. 48 (8): 517–25. PMID 14574827. Retrieved 2007-06-27.
  82. ^ Williams JG, Higgins JPT, Brayne CEG (2006). "Systematic review of prevalence studies of autism spectrum disorders". Arch Dis Child. 91 (1): 8–15. doi:10.1136/adc.2004.062083. PMID 15863467. Retrieved 2007-07-19.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  83. ^ Fombonne E (2005). "Epidemiology of autistic disorder and other pervasive developmental disorders". J Clin Psychiatry. 66 (Suppl 10): 3–8. PMID 16401144.
  84. ^ Baird G, Simonoff E, Pickles A; et al. (2006). "Prevalence of disorders of the autism spectrum in a population cohort of children in South Thames: the Special Needs and Autism Project (SNAP)". Lancet. 368 (9531): 210–5. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(06)69041-7. PMID 16844490. {{cite journal}}: Explicit use of et al. in: |author= (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  85. ^ Kolevzon A, Gross R, Reichenberg A (2007). "Prenatal and perinatal risk factors for autism". Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 161 (4): 326–33. PMID 17404128. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  86. ^ Zafeiriou DI, Ververi A, Vargiami E (2007). "Childhood autism and associated comorbidities". Brain Dev. 29 (5): 257–72. doi:10.1016/j.braindev.2006.09.003. PMID 17084999.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  87. ^ Chakrabarti S, Fombonne E (2001). "Pervasive developmental disorders in preschool children". JAMA. 285 (24): 3093–9. PMID 11427137. Retrieved 2007-07-18.
  88. ^ Tuchman R, Rapin I (2002). "Epilepsy in autism". Lancet Neurol. 1 (6): 352–8. doi:10.1016/S1474-4422(02)00160-6. PMID 12849396.
  89. ^ Matson JL, Nebel-Schwalm MS (2007). "Comorbid psychopathology with autism spectrum disorder in children: an overview". Res Dev Disabil. 28 (4): 341–52. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2005.12.004. PMID 16765022.
  90. ^ Changes in diagnostic practices:
    • Fombonne E (2003). "The prevalence of autism". JAMA. 289 (1): 87–9. PMID 12503982.
    • Wing L, Potter D (2002). "The epidemiology of autistic spectrum disorders: is the prevalence rising?". Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. 8 (3): 151–61. doi:10.1002/mrdd.10029. PMID 12216059.
  91. ^ Byrd RS, Sage AC, Keyzer J; et al. (2002). "Report to the legislature on the principal findings of the epidemiology of autism in California: a comprehensive pilot study" (PDF). M.I.N.D. Institute. Retrieved 2006-09-18. {{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help); Explicit use of et al. in: |author= (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  92. ^ Shattuck PT (2006). "The contribution of diagnostic substitution to the growing administrative prevalence of autism in US special education". Pediatrics. 117 (4): 1028–37. doi:10.1542/peds.2005-1516. PMID 16585296. Retrieved 2007-07-18.
  93. ^ Stenson J (2005-02-24). "As autism cases soar, a search for clues". MSNBC. Retrieved 2007-06-26. {{cite news}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  94. ^ Wing L (1997). "The history of ideas on autism: legends, myths and reality". Autism. 1 (1): 13–23. doi:10.1177/1362361397011004.
  95. ^ Miles M (2005). "Martin Luther and childhood disability in 16th century Germany: what did he write? what did he say?". Independent Living Institute. Retrieved 2007-07-18.
  96. ^ a b c Wolff S (2004). "The history of autism". Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 13 (4): 201–8. doi:10.1007/s00787-004-0363-5. PMID 15365889.
  97. ^ Kuhn R; tr. Cahn CH (2004). "Eugen Bleuler's concepts of psychopathology". Hist Psychiatry. 15 (3): 361–6. doi:10.1177/0957154X04044603. PMID 15386868.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) The quote is a translation of Bleuler's 1910 original.
  98. ^ Fombonne E (2003). "Modern views of autism". Can J Psychiatry. 48 (8): 503–5. PMID 14574825. Retrieved 2007-07-05.
  99. ^ Biever C (2007-06-27). "Web removes social barriers for those with autism". New Scientist. No. 2610. Retrieved 2007-06-28. {{cite news}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)

External links

Listen to this article
(4 parts, 45 minutes)
  1. Part 1
  2. Part 4
Spoken Wikipedia icon
These audio files were created from a revision of this article dated
Error: no date provided
, and do not reflect subsequent edits.